Skip to main content
Log in

Covert judgements are sufficient to trigger subsequent task-switching costs

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This research examines whether we have a tendency to repeat mental processes leading to decisions or judgements that are not accompanied by overt behaviours. We adapted the task-switching paradigm so that on selected trials task processing would be terminated prior to response execution. Switch costs were present subsequent to trials where task processing was terminated either at the stage of response selection or at the earlier stage of making a covert judgement (a mental decision) about the target stimulus. These costs were residual, as they occurred despite long preparation intervals, and they did not result from cue-switching or feature-repetition effects. We conclude that the same type of control mechanism may be recruited to select between potential alternative tasks whenever a stimulus needs to be processed in a task-specific way, regardless of whether or not an overt response is required.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allport, A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umlita & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XV (pp. 421–452). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, A., & Wylie, G. R. (1999). Task-switching: Positive and negative priming of task-set. In G. W. Humphreys, J. Duncan, & A. Treisman (Eds.), Attention, space and action: Studies in cognitive neuroscience (pp. 273–296). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, A., & Wylie, G. R. (2000). Task switching, stimulus-response bindings and negative priming. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 35–70). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Altmann, E. M. (2004). The preparation effect in task switching: Carryover of SOA. Memory and Cognition, 32(1), 153–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Astle, D. E., Jackson, G. M., & Swainson, R. (2006). Dissociating neural indices of dynamic cognitive control in advance task-set preparation: An ERP study of task switching. Brain Research, 1125, 94–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Astle, D. E., Jackson, G. M., & Swainson, R. (2012). Two measures of task-specific inhibition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 2006, 1–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bertelson, P. (1965). Serial choice reaction-time as a function of response versus signal-and-response repetition. Nature, 206, 217–218.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brass, M., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2004). Decomposing components of task preparation with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(4), 609–620.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, R. (2000). An intention-activation account of residual switch costs. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 357–376). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hübner, M., Kluwe, R. H., Luna-Rodriquez, A., & Peters, A. (2004). Response selection difficulty and asymmetrical costs of switching between tasks and stimuli: No evidence for an exogenous component of task-set reconfiguration. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30(6), 1043–1063.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kühn, S., & Brass, M. (2010). The cognitive representation of intending not to act: Evidence for specific non-action-effect binding. Cognition, 117(1), 9–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kühn, S., Elsner, B., Prinz, W., & Brass, M. (2009). Busy doing nothing: Evidence for nonaction—effect binding. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(3), 542–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lenartowicz, A., Yeung, N., & Cohen, J. D. (2011). No-go trials can modulate switch cost by interfering with effects of task preparation. Psychological Research, 75(1), 66–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D., & Bundesen, C. (2003). Clever homunculus: Is there an endogenous act of control in the explicit task-cuing procedure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(3), 575–599.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, U., & Keele, S. W. (2000). Changing internal constraints on action: The role of backward inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129(1), 4–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meiran, N. (2000). Reconfiguration of stimulus task sets and response task sets during task switching. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 377–399). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 134–140.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pashler, H., & Baylis, G. (1991). Procedural learning 2: Intertrial repetition effects in speeded-choice tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 33–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Philipp, A. M., Jolicoeur, P., Falkenstein, M., & Koch, I. (2007). Response selection and response execution in task switching: Evidence from a go-signal paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(6), 1062–1075.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubinstein, J. S., Meyer, D. E., & Evans, J. E. (2001). Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(4), 763–797.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schuch, S., & Koch, I. (2003). The role of response selection for inhibition of task sets in task shifting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(1), 92–105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sohn, M. H., Ursu, S., Anderson, J. R., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2000). The role of prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex in task-switching. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97(24), 13448–13453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steinhauser, M. (2010). How to correct a task error: Task-switch effects following different types of error correction. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(4), 1028–1035.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steinhauser, M., & Hübner, R. (2006). Response-based strengthening in task shifting: Evidence from shift effects produced by errors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(3), 517–534.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Verbruggen, F., Liefooghe, B., & Vandierendonck, A. (2006). Selective stopping in task switching: The role of response selection and response execution. Experimental Psychology, 53(1), 48–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Verbruggen, F., Liefooghe, B., Vandierendonck, A., & Demanet, J. (2007). Short cue presentations encourage advance task preparation: A recipe to diminish the residual switch cost. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(2), 342–356.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Steve Harley, Julie Main, Sindre Henriksen, Dudu Ozlevent and Maritxu Arlegui-Prieto for their assistance with the data collection. We would like to thank Mike Wendt and an anonymous reviewer for their helpful comments on the manuscript. This work was funded by an Experimental Psychology Society Small Grant.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rachel Swainson.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Swainson, R., Martin, D. Covert judgements are sufficient to trigger subsequent task-switching costs. Psychological Research 77, 434–448 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-012-0448-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-012-0448-6

Keywords

Navigation