Skip to main content
Log in

Do action goals mediate social inhibition of return?

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Social inhibition of return is the phenomenon whereby an individual is slower to reach to locations to which another individual has recently responded. Although this suggests that an observer represents another person’s action, little is known about which aspects of the action are encoded. The present work describes a series of three experiments examining whether social inhibition of return represents the endpoint goal of the action, i.e., is ‘goal based’. Pairs of participants sat opposite to one another and alternated responses to a cued or non-cued object presented on a table top. Importantly, either the two participants performed the same interaction with the object or a different interaction. Although all our experiments showed social inhibition of return, the size of the effect was not modulated according to whether each participant had the same or different goal. We conclude that although the mechanisms giving rise to social inhibition of return do encode some aspects of a response they do not code for terminal action goals.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abrams, R. A., & Christ, S. E. (2003). Motion onset captures attention. Psychological Science, 14, 427–432.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bekkering, H., Wohlschläger, A., & Gattis, G. (2000). Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 53(A), 153–164.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, A., & Henik, A. (2000). The endogenous modulation of IOR is nasal-temporal asymmetric. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 421–428.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brennan, S. E., Chen, X., Dickinson, C. A., Neider, M. B., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2008). Coordinating cognition: The costs and benefits of shared gaze during collaborative search. Cognition, 106, 1465–1477.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Fourteen- through 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 315–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cattaneo, L., Fabbri-Destro, M., Boria, S., Pieraccini, C., Monti, A., Cossu, G., et al. (2007). Impairment of actions chains in autism and its possible role in intention understanding. Proceedings of the Nation Academy of Sciences, 104, 17825–17830.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cave, K. R., Bush, W. S., & Taylor, T. G. G. (2010). Split attention as part of a flexible attentional system for complex scenes: Comment on Jans, Peters, and De Weerd (2010). Psychological Review, 117, 685–696.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, G. G., Gellatly, A. R. H., & Blurton, A. (2001). Effect of object onset on the distribution of visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 1356–1368.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, G. G., & Kuhn, G. (2009). Appearance matters: Attentional orienting by new objects in the precuing paradigm. Visual Cognition, 17, 755–776.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, G. G., & Kuhn, G. (2010). What the experimenter’s prime tells the observer’s brain. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 1367–1376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, G. G., Kuhn, G., & Liversedge, S. P. (2007). Onset of illusory figures attenuates change blindness. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 939–942.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, G. G., & Liversedge, S. L. (2006). Change blindness and the primacy of object appearance. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 588–593.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Croson, R., & Sundali, J. (2005). The Gambler’s fallacy and the hot hand: Empirical data from casinos. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 30, 195–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daprati, E., & Sirigu, A. (2006). How we interact with objects: Learning from brain lesions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 265–270.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events: A neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research, 91, 176–180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (1998). Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1737–1747.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P., Gesierich, B., Rozzi, S., Chaersi, F., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Parietal lobe: From action organization to intention understanding. Science, 308, 662–667.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gattis, M., Bekkering, H., & Wohlschläger, A. (2002). Goal-directed imitation. In A. N. Meltzoff & W. Prinz (Eds.), The Imitative Mind (pp. 183–205). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. J., Hansen, S., & Elliot, D. (2009). Between-person effects on attention and action: Joe and Fred revisited. Psychological Research, 74, 302–312.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. (2009). Action control according to TEC (theory of event coding). Psychological Research, 73, 512–526.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod, M. (1999). The 25th Bartlett Lecture: To act or not to act: Perspectives on the representation of actions. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52, 1–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Khatoon, S., Briand, K. A., & Sereno, A. B. (2002). The role of response in spatial attention: Direct versus indirect stimulus–response mappings. Vision Research, 42, 2693–2708.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kingstone, A., & Pratt, J. (1999). Inhibition of return is composed of attentional and oculomotor processes. Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 1046–1054.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, R. M., & MacInnes, W. J. (1999). Inhibition of return is a foraging facilitator in visual search. Psychological Science, 10, 346–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, G., Tatler, B. W., & Cole, G. G. (2009). You look where I look! Effect of gaze cues on overt and covert attention in misdirection. Visual Cognition, 17, 925–944.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lupiàñez, J., & Milliken, B. (1999). Inhibition of return and the attentional set for integrating vs. differentiating information. Journal of General Psychology, Theme Issue on Visual Attention, Part 2, 126, 392–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lupiàñez, J., Milliken, B., Solano, C., Weaver, B., & Tipper, S. P. (2001). On the strategic modulation of the time course of facilitation and inhibition of return. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 753–773.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, A. R., Seidenberg, M., Dorflinger, J. M., & Rao, S. M. (2004). An event-related fMRI study of exogenous orienting: Supporting evidence for the cortical basis of inhibition of return? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1262–1271.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mukamel, R., Ekstrom, A. D., Kaplan, J., Iacoboni, M., & Fried, I. (2010). Single-neuron responses in humans during execution and observation of actions. Current Biology, 20, 750–756.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. G. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and Performance X (pp. 531–556). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

  • Posner, M. I., Rafal, R. D., Choate, L. S., & Vaughan, J. (1985). Inhibition of return: Neural basis and function. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 2, 211–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rieger, M., & Gauggel, S. (1999). Inhibitory after-effects in the stop-signal paradigm. British Journal of Psychology, 90, 509–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riess-Jones, M., Moynihan, H., MacKenzie, N., & Puente, J. (2002). Temporal aspects of stimulus-driven attending in dynamic arrays. Psychological Science, 13, 313–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Fabbri-Destro, M., & Cattaneo, L. (2008). Mirror neurons and their clinical relevance. Nature Clinical Practice Neurology, 5, 24–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubichi, S., Nicoletti, R., Iani, C., & Umilta, C. (1997). The simon effect occurs relative to the direction of an attentional shift. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1353–1364.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schuch, S., & Tipper, S. P. (2007). On observing another person’s actions: Influences of observed inhibition and errors. Perception & Psychophysics, 69, 828–837.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2003). Representing others’ actions: Just like one’s own? Cognition, 88, 11–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 174–176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sirigu, A., Cohen, L., Duhamel, J. R., Pillon, B., Dubois, B., & Agid, Y. (1995). A selective impairment of hand posture for object utilisation in apraxia. Cortex, 31, 41–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skarratt, P. A., Cole, G. G., & Kingstone, A. (2010). Social inhibition of return. Acta Psychologica, 134, 48–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki, S., & Cavanagh, P. (1997). Focused attention distorts visual space: A attentional repulsion effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 443–463.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, T. L., & Klein, R. M. (2000). Visual and motor effects in inhibition of return. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 1639–1656.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tipper, C., & Kingstone, A. (2005). Is inhibition of return a reflexive effect? Cognition, 97, B55–B62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, T. N., Elliot, D., Anson, J. G., Dhillon, V., Weeks, D. J., Lyons, J. L., et al. (2005). Does Joe influence Fred’s actions? Inhibition of return across different nervous systems. Neuroscience Letters, 385, 99–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, T. N., Lyons, J., Weeks, D. J., Anson, J. G., Chua, R., Mendoza, J., & Elliot, D. (2007). Within- and between-person inhibition of return: Observation is as good as performance. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 960–956.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, T. N., McDougall, L. M., & Weeks, D. J. (2009a). The performance and observation of action shape future behaviour. Brain and Cognition, 71, 64–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, T. N., Ray, M. C., Weeks, D. J., Deweyd, D., & Elliott, D. (2009b). Does Joe influence Fred’s action? Not if Fred has autism spectrum disorder. Brain Research, 1248, 141–148.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by UK Economic and Social Research Council Grant RES-000-22-1766 and a research development grant from the University of Essex. The authors thank the reviewers for their assistance on earlier drafts of this article.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Geoff G. Cole.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Cole, G.G., Skarratt, P.A. & Billing, RC. Do action goals mediate social inhibition of return?. Psychological Research 76, 736–746 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0395-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0395-7

Keywords

Navigation