Skip to main content
Log in

Does observed fertility maximize fitness among New Mexican men?

A test of an optimality model and a new theory of parental investment in the embodied capital of offspring

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Our objective is to test an optimality model of human fertility that specifies the behavioral requirements for fitness maximization in order (a) to determine whether current behavior does maximize fitness and, if not, (b) to use the specific nature of the behavioral deviations from fitness maximization towards the development of models of evolved proximate mechanisms that may have maximized fitness in the past but lead to deviations under present conditions. To test the model we use data from a representative sample of 7,107 men living in Albuquerque, New Mexico, between 1990 and 1993. The model we test proposes that low fertility in modern settings maximizes number of grandchildren as a result of a trade-off between parental fertility and next generation fertility. Results do not show the optimization, although the data do reveal a trade-off between parental fertility and offspring education and income.

We propose that two characteristics of modern economies have led to a period of sustained fertility reduction and to a corresponding lack of association between income and fertility. The first is the direct link between costs of investment and wage rates due to the forces of supply and demand for labor in competitive economies. The second is the increasing emphasis on cumulative knowledge, skills, and technologies in the production of resources. Together they produce historically novel conditions. These two features of modern economies may interact with evolved psychological and physiological mechanisms governing fertility and parental investment to produce behavior that maximizes the economic productivity of lineages at the expense of fitness. If cognitive processes evolved to track diminishing returns to parental investment and if physiological processes evolved to regulate fertility in response to nutritional state and patterns of breast feeding, we might expect non-adaptive responses when returns from parental investment do not diminish until extremely high levels are reached. With high economic payoffs from parental investment, people have begun to exercise cognitive regulation of fertility through contraception and family planning practices. Those cognitive processes maynot have evolved to handle fitness trade-offs between fertility and parental investment.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barkow, J. H., and N. Burley 1980 Human Fertility, Evolutionary Biology, and the Demographic Transition.Ethology and Sociobiology 1:163–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnett, W. S. 1985The Perry Preschool Program and Its Long-Term Effects: A Benefit-Cost Analysis. Early Childhood Policy Papers No. 2. Ypsilanti, Michigan: High/Scope Educational Research Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, G. S. 1975Human Capital, second ed. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • 1981A Treatise on the Family. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, G. S., and N. Tomes 1976 Child Endowments and the Quantity and Quality of Children.Journal of Political Economy 84:s143-s163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becker, G. S., K. M. Murphy, and R. Tamura 1990 Human Capital, Fertility and Economic Growth.Journal of Political Economy 98:s12-s37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Betzig, L. L. 1986Despotism and Differential Reproduction: A Darwinian View of History. Hawthorne, New York: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blurton Jones, N. G. 1986 Bushman Birth Spacing: A Test for Optimal Interbirth Intervals.Ethology and Sociobiology 7:91–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blurton Jones, N. G., and R. M. Sibly 1978 Testing Adaptiveness of Culturally Determined Behavior: Do Bushman Women Maximize Their Reproductive Success by Spacing Births Widely and Foraging Seldom? InHuman Behavior and Adaptation, N. G. Blurton Jones and V. Reynolds, eds. Pp. 135–157. London: Taylor and Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borgerhoff Mulder, M. 1992 Reproductive Decisions. InEvolutionary Ecology and Human Behavior, E. A. Smith and B. Winterhalder, eds. Pp. 339–374. Hawthorne, New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charnov, E. 1993Life History Invariants: Some Explorations of Symmetry in Evolutionary Ecology. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Consortium for Longitudinal Studies 1983As the Twig Is Bent: Lasting Effects of Preschool Programs. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cromer, A. 1993Uncommon Sense: The Heretical Nature of Science. New York: Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronk, L. 1991 Human Behavioral Ecology.Annual Reviews in Anthropology 20:25–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Easterlin, R. A. 1987Birth and Fortune: The Impact of Numbers on Personal Welfare, second ed. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, B., and T. Risley 1995Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children. Baltimore: Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henry, L. 1961 Some Data on Natural Fertility.Eugenics Quarterly 8:81–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K., and A. M. Hurtado 1995Ache Life History: The Ecology and Demography of a Foraging People. Hawthorne, New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoff-Ginsberg, E., and T. Tardif 1995 Socioeconomic Status and Parenting. InThe Handbook of Parenting, vol. 2, M. Bornstein, ed. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holekamp, K. E., and L. Smale 1991 Dominance Acquisition during Mammalian Social Development: The Inheritance of Maternal Rank.American Zoologist 31:306–317.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hood, J. 1992Caveat Emptor: The Head Start Scam. Policy Analysis No. 187. Washington, D.C.: Cato Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howell, N. 1979Demography of the Dobe !Kung. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irons, W. 1983 Human Female Reproductive Strategies. InSocial Behavior of Female Vertebrates, S. K. Wasser, ed. Pp. 169–213. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • 1990 Let’s Make Our Perspectives Broader Rather Than Narrower: A Comment on Turke’s “Which Humans Behave Adaptively, and Why Does It Matter?” and on the So-called DA-DP Debate.Ethology and Sociobiology 11:361–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • 1993 Monogamy, Contraception, and the Cultural and Reproductive Success Hypothesis.Behavior and Brain Sciences 16:295–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • 1995 Cultural and Reproductive Success in Traditional Societies. Ms. in author’s possession, Department of Anthropology, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, H. 1994a Evolutionary and Wealth Flows Theories of Fertility: Empirical Tests and New Models.Population and Development Review 20:753–791.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • 1994b A Theory of Human Parental Investment and Fertility. Ms. in author’s possession, Department of Anthropology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, H. S., J. B. Lancaster, J. A. Bock, and S. E. Johnson 1995 Fertility and Fitness among Albuquerque Men: A Competitive Labour Market Theory. InHuman Reproductive Decisions: Biological and Social Perspectives, R. I. M. Dunbar, ed. Pp. 96–136. New York: St. Martin’s Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lack, D. 1954The Natural Regulation of Animal Numbers. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • 1968Ecological Adaptations for Breeding in Birds. London: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lam, D. 1986 The Dynamics of Population Growth, Differential Fertility, and Inequality.American Economic Review 76:1103–1116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lancaster, J. B. in press An Evolutionary History of Human Reproductive Strategies and the Status of Women in Relation to Population Growth and Social Stratification. InEvolution and Feminism, P. A. Gowaty, ed. New York: Chapman and Hall.

  • Lancaster, J. B., and C. S. Lancaster 1987 The Watershed: Change in Parental-Investment and Family-Formation Strategies in the Course of Human Evolution. InParenting across the Life Span: Biosocial Dimensions, J. B. Lancaster, J. Altmann, A. S. Rossi, and L. R. Sherrod, eds. Pp. 187–205. Hawthorne, New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, P. C., P. Majluf, and I. J. Gordon 1991 Growth, Weaning and Maternal Investment from a Comparative Perspective. Journal of Zoology (London) 225:99–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perusse, D. 1993 Cultural and Reproductive Success in Industrial Societies: Testing the Relationship at the Proximate and Ultimate Levels (with comments).Brain and Behavioral Sciences 16:267–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Retherford, R. D. 1993 Demographic Transition and the Evolution of Intelligence: Theory and Evidence. Ms. in author’s possession, East-West Center, Honolulu, Hawaii.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, A. R. 1990 Evolutionary Economics of Human Reproduction.Ethology and Sociobiology 11:479–495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • 1993 Why Menopause?Evolutionary Ecology 7:406–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, A. R., and N. G. Blurton Jones 1992 Allocation of Parental Care. Ms. in authors’ possession, Department of Anthropology, University of Utah, Salt Lake City.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schweinhart, L. J., H. V. Barnes, and D. P. Weikart 1993Significant Benefits: The High/Scope Perry Preschool Study through Age 27. Monographs of the High/Scope Education Research Foundation, No. 10. Ypsilanti, Michigan: High/Scope Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simons, J. L. 1974The Effects of Income on Fertility. Chapel Hill, North Carolina: Carolina Populations Center.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. C., and S. D. Fretwell 1974 The Optimal Balance between Size and Number of Offspring.American Naturalist 108:499–506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stearns, S. C. 1992The Evolution of Life Histories. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turke, P. W. 1989 Evolution and the Demand for Children.Population and Development Review 15:61–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • U. S. Department of Commerce 1992 U. S. Census of Population: General Population Characteristics: New Mexico. Washington, D.C.: Bureau of the Census.

    Google Scholar 

  • U. S. Census of Population and Housing, New Mexico 1991a Summary Tape File 1, New Mexico (1990). Washington, D.C.: Bureau of the Census.

    Google Scholar 

  • 1991b Summary Tape File 3, New Mexico (1990). Washington, D.C.: Bureau of the Census.

    Google Scholar 

  • 1991c CP-1-33, Table 54, Pg. 82. Washington, D.C.: Government Printing Office.

    Google Scholar 

  • U. S. Department of Transportation, Federal Highway Administration. 1993 Highway statistics 1987. TD2.22.991. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Schaik, C. P. 1989 The Ecology of Social Relationships amongst Female Primates. InComparative Socioecology, V. Standen and R. A. Foley, eds. Pp. 195–218. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vining, D. R. 1986 Social versus Reproductive Success: The Central Theoretical Problem of Human Sociobiology.Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9:167–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vitzthum, V. 1994 Comparative Study of Breastfeeding Structure and Its Relation to Human Reproductive Ecology.Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 37:307–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zigler, E., and S. J. Styfco 1994 Head Start: Criticisms in a Constructive Context.American Psychologist 49:127–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hillard S. Kaplan.

Additional information

A preliminary presentation of this data was published in R. I. M. Dunbar, ed.,Human Reproduction Decisions: Biological and Social Perspectives. New York: St. Martin’s Press, 1995. Support for the research project, “Male Fertility and Parenting in New Mexico,” began with two seed grants from the University of New Mexico’s Biomedical Research Grants Program, 1988 and 1989, and one from the University of New Mexico Research Allocations Committee, 1988. Further seed money as well as interim funding came from the William T. Grant Foundation (#89130589 and #91130501). The major support for the project came from the National Science Foundation from 1990 to 1993 (#BNS-9011723 and #DBS-911552). Both National Science Foundation grants included Research Experience for Undergraduates supplements.

Hillard S. Kaplan is an Associate Professor of Anthropology at the University of New Mexico. His earlier research and publications focused on food sharing, time allocation, parental investment, and reproductive strategies among Ache hunter-gatherers in Paraguay, Machiguenga and Piro forager-horticulturalists in Peru, and villagers of several ethnicities in Botswana. New research and theory concern fertility, parental investment, and mating strategies in developed and developing nations. This research formulates a new theory of reproductive decision-making and the demographic transition, integrating human capital and parental investment theory in a synthesis of economic and evolutionary approaches.

Jane B. Lancaster is a Professor of Anthropology at the University of New Mexico. Her research and publications are on human reproductive biology and behavior, especially human parental investment; women’s reproductive biology of pregnancy, lactation, and child-spacing; and male fertility and investment in children. Current research with Hillard S. Kaplan is on male life history strategies among a large sample of men in New Mexico. She has coedited three books on human parental investment:School-Age Pregnancy and Parenthood (with B. Hamburg),Parenting across the Life Span (with J. Altmann, A. Rossi, and L. Sherrod), andOffspring Abuse and Neglect (with R. Gelles). She is scientific editor of a quarterly journal,Human Nature: An Interdisciplinary, Biosocial Perspective published by Aldine de Gruyter. She is also a council member of the newly formed Human Behavior and Evolution Society.

John A. Bock is Andrew W. Mellon Post-Doctoral Fellow in Epidemiology and Population Health at the National Centre for Epidemiology and Population Health, The Australian National University. His research focuses on the allocation of parental investment and the determinants of children’s activities, integrating aspects of economic and evolutionary theory. He has ongoing field research with Bantu and Bushmen agro-pastoralists and forager-horticulturalists in the Okavango Delta, Botswana. He is also collaborating with Lancaster and Kaplan on the determinants of progeny distribution and homosexuality among New Mexican men.

Sara E. Johnson is a Ph.D. candidate at the University of New Mexico. Her major research trajectory focuses on trade-offs in life history characters. Her research experience includes participation in a study of variation in growth and development among children in a multi-ethnic community in the Okavango Delta, Botswana, in addition to her dissertation work on individual variation in growth and mortality among juvenile baboons. She is collaborating with Lancaster and Kaplan on the association between survival and fertility among Albuquerque men.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kaplan, H.S., Lancaster, J.B., Johnson, S.E. et al. Does observed fertility maximize fitness among New Mexican men?. Human Nature 6, 325–360 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02734205

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02734205

Key words

Navigation