Skip to main content
Log in

Increased choice of female phonetic attributes in first names

  • Published:
Sex Roles Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The phonetic gender score is a new quantitative scale that was applied to the spoken sounds of first names. Popular names of females have predominantly positive scores, and popular names of males have predominantly negative scores. Mean phonetic gender scores were higher in 1990 than in 1960 for the 25 most frequent names given to females and males born in Pennsylvania. Choices of names were more diverse for females than males in both years and in 1990 than 1960 for both genders. The increased choice in 1990 of attributes associated with females may indicate greater acceptance of female characteristics in 1990 than in 1960. In 1990 the most numerous racial minority, African Americans, constituted 15% of the births but only 5% of the females and 7% of the males given the 25 most frequent names.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, C. H., & Sherer, M. (1985). Sex-role orientation and psychological adjustment: implications for the masculinity model.Sex Roles, 12, 1211–1218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barry, H., III, Bacon, M. K. & Child, I. L. (1957). A cross-cultural survey of some sex differences in socialization.Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 55, 327–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barry, H., III, & Harper, A. S. (1982). Evolution of unisex names.Names, 30, 15–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barry, H., III, & Harper, A. S. (1993a). Sex differences in linguistic origins of personal names. In E. W. McMullen (Ed.),Names new and old. Madison, NJ: Penny Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barry, H., III, & Harper, A. S. (1993b). Feminization of unisex names from 1960 to 1990.Names, 41, 228–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bem, S. L. (1993).The lenses of gender. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bergen, D. J., & Williams, J. E. (1991). Sex stereotypes in the United States revisited: 1972–1988.Sex Roles, 24, 413–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks-Gunn, J., & Fisch, M. (1980). Psychological androgyny and college students' judgments of mental health.Sex Roles, 6, 575–580.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duffy, J. C., & Ridinger, B. (1981). Stereotyped connotations of masculine and feminine names.Sex Roles, 7, 25–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Etaugh, C., & Liss, M. B. (1992). Home, school, and playroom: training grounds for adult gender roles.Sex Roles, 26, 129–147.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garwood, S. G., Baer, S., Levine, D., Carroll, S., & O'Neal, E. (1981). Sex-role expectations of socially desirable first names.Sex Roles, 7, 257–262.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkins, P. (1984).Introducing phonology. London: Hutchinson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hort, B. E., Fagot, B. I., & Leinbach, M. D. (1990). Are people's notions of maleness more stereotypically framed than their notions of femaleness?Sex Roles, 23, 197–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberson, S., & Bell, E. O. (1992). Children's first names: an empirical study of social taste.American Journal of Sociology, 98, 511–554.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L. (1990). Attitudes and expectations about children with nontraditional and traditional gender roles.Sex Roles, 22, 151–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennsylvania Statistical Abstract (1961). (14th ed.). Harrisburg: Pennsylvania Bureau of Publications.

  • Pennsylvania Vital Statistics Annual Report, 1990. Harrisburg: Pennsylvania State Health Data Center.

  • Rossi, A. S. (1965). Naming children in middle-class families.American Sociological Review, 30, 499–513.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwegel, J. (1988).The Baby Name Countdown: Popularity and Meaning of Today's Baby Names. Edmonton, Alberta, Canada: Personal Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slater, A. S., & Feinman, S. (1985). Gender and the phonology of North American first names.Sex Roles, 13, 429–440.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. C. (1950).The story of our names. New York: Harper.

    Google Scholar 

  • SPSS Inc. (1986).SPSSX User's Guide (2nd ed.). Chicago: SPSS.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werner, P. D., & LaRussa, G. W. (1985). Persistence and change in sex-role stereotypes.Sex Roles, 12, 1089–1100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitley, B. E., Jr. (1984). Sex-role orientation and psychological well-being: two meta-analyses.Sex Roles, 12, 207–225.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

The data on frequencies of first names were supplied by the State Center for Health Statistics and Research, Pennsylvania Department of Health, Harrisburg, Pennsylvania. The Department specifically disclaims responsibility for any analyses, interpretations, or conclusions.

Helpful services were received from the Pennsylvania State Center for Health Statistics and Research, in particular by Jerry Orris, and from the University of Pittsburgh Computer Center, in particular data transfer and programming by Eileen S. Kopchik and statistical consultation by Clement A. Stone.

A portion of this study was reported at the 33rd Names Institute, Baruch College, New York, on May 7, 1994.

Community College of Allegheny County

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Barry, H., Harper, A.S. Increased choice of female phonetic attributes in first names. Sex Roles 32, 809–819 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01560190

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01560190

Keywords

Navigation