Skip to main content
Log in

Early Callous-Unemotional Behavior, Theory-of-Mind, and a Fearful/Inhibited Temperament Predict Externalizing Problems in Middle and Late Childhood

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Childhood externalizing problems are more likely to be severe and persistent when combined with high levels of callous-unemotional (CU) behavior. A handful of recent studies have shown that CU behavior can also be reliably measured in the early preschool years, which may help to identify young children who are less likely to desist from early externalizing behaviors. The current study extends previous literature by examining the role of CU behavior in very early childhood in the prediction of externalizing problems in both middle and late childhood, and tests whether other relevant child characteristics, including Theory-of-Mind (ToM) and fearful/inhibited temperament moderate these pathways. Multi-method data, including parent reports of child CU behavior and fearful/inhibited temperament, observations of ToM, and teacher-reported externalizing problems were drawn from a prospective, longitudinal study of children assessed at ages 3, 6, and 10 (N = 241; 48 % female). Results demonstrated that high levels of CU behavior predicted externalizing problems at ages 6 and 10 over and above the effect of earlier externalizing problems at age 3, but that these main effects were qualified by two interactions. High CU behavior was related to higher levels of externalizing problems specifically for children with low ToM and a low fearful/inhibited temperament. The results show that a multitude of child characteristics likely interact across development to increase or buffer risk for child externalizing problems. These findings can inform the development of targeted early prevention and intervention for children with high CU behavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We chose to explore ToM as a moderator of the relationship between CU behavior and later externalizing problems. However, we acknowledge that an alternative conceptualization of these models is to consider CU behavior as the moderator. In this case, when probing the interaction term, we find a trend-level negative effect of ToM at age 3 on later externalizing problems at age 6 only when children had high levels of CU behavior, b = −0.77 (0.39), t = −1.97, p < 0.10, but not when they had low levels of CU behavior, b = 0.27 (0.27), t = 0.99, ns. While interesting to consider CU behavior as a moderator, especially to explain the inconsistent findings that have been reported by studies examining the relationship between ToM and later externalizing problems (Hughes et al. 2000; Olson et al. 2011), we note that our goal was to examine early CU behavior as a predictor and the potential emotional and cognitive moderating factors that influenced the strength of its relationship with later externalizing problems.

References

  • Achenbach, T. M. (1992). Manual for the child behavior checklist/2–3 and 1992 profile. Burlington: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.

    Google Scholar 

  • Achenbach, T. M. (1997). Teacher/caregiver report form for ages 2–5. Burlington: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.

    Google Scholar 

  • Achenbach, T. M., Dumenci, L., & Rescorla, L. A. (2002). Ten-year comparisons of problems and competencies for national samples of youth: self, parent and teacher reports. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 10, 194–203. doi:10.1177/10634266020100040101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for ASEBA school-Age forms & profiles. Burlington: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ahadi, S. A., Rothbart, M. K., & Ye, R. (1993). Children's temperament in the US and China: similarities and differences. European Journal of Personality, 7, 359–377. doi:10.1002/per.2410070506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: testing and interpreting interactions. CA: Sage Publications, Inc..

    Google Scholar 

  • Asendorpf, J. B., van de Schoot, R., Denissen, J. A., & Hutteman, R. (2014). Reducing bias due to systematic attrition in longitudinal studies: the benefits of multiple imputation. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38, 453–460. doi:10.1177/0165025414542713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Astington, J. W. (2001). The future of theory-of-mind research: understanding motivational states, the role of language, and real-world consequences. Child Development, 72, 685–687. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00305.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Astington, J. W. (2003). Sometimes necessary, never sufficient: false-belief understanding and social competence. In B. Repacholi, & V. Slaughter (Eds.), Individual differences in theory of mind: implications for typical and atypical development (pp. 13–38). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. (1989). Young children's attribution of action to beliefs and desires. Child Development, 60, 946–964. doi:10.2307/1131035.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berg, J. M., Lilienfeld, S. O., Reddy, S. D., Latzman, R. D., Roose, A., Craighead, L. W., et al. (2013). The inventory of callous and unemotional traits: a construct-validational analysis in an at-risk sample. Assessment, 20, 532–544. doi:10.1177/1073191112474338.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. R. (1995). A cognitive developmental approach to morality: investigating the psychopath. Cognition, 57, 1–29. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(95)00676-P.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. R. (1999). Responsiveness to distress cues in the child with psychopathic tendencies. Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 135–145. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00231-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (1995). The continuity of maladaptive behavior: from description to understanding in the study of antisocial behavior. In D. Cicchetti, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology, Risk, disorder, and adaptation (vol. 2, pp. 472–511). England: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Choe, D. E., Lane, J. D., Grabell, A. S., & Olson, S. L. (2013). Developmental precursors of young school-age children’s hostile attribution bias. Developmental Psychology, 49, 2245–2256. doi:10.1037/a0032293.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dadds, M. R., Cauchi, A. J., Wimalaweera, S., Hawes, D. J., & Brennan, J. (2012). Outcomes, moderators, and mediators of empathic-emotion recognition training for complex conduct problems in childhood. Psychiatry Research, 199, 201–207. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2012.04.033.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dadds, M. R., Hawes, D. J., Frost, A. D., Vassallo, S., Bunn, P., Hunter, K., & Merz, S. (2009). Learning to ‘talk the talk’: the relationship of psychopathic traits to deficits in empathy across childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 599–606. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.02058.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dadds, M. R., & Salmon, K. (2003). Punishment insensitivity and parenting: temperament and learning as interacting risks for antisocial behavior. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 6, 69–86. doi:10.1023/A:1023762009877.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1987). Social-information-processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children's peer groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1146–1158. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.53.6.1146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dodge, K. A., Greenberg, M. T., & Malone, P. S. (2008). Testing an idealized dynamic cascade model of the development of serious violence in adolescence. Child Development, 79, 1907–1927. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01233.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Essau, C. A., Sasagawa, S., & Frick, P. J. (2006). Callous-unemotional traits in a community sample of adolescents. Assessment, 13, 454–469. doi:10.1177/1073191106287354.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fanti, K. A., Panayiotou, G., Lazarou, C., Michael, R., & Georgiou, G. (2015). The better of two evils? Evidence that children exhibiting continuous conduct problems high or low on callous–unemotional traits score on opposite directions on physiological and behavioral measures of fear. Development and Psychopathology Advance Online publication. doi:10.1017/S0954579415000371.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feinstein, L., & Bynner, J. (2006). Continuity and discontinuity in middle childhood: implications for adult outcomes in the UK 1970 birth cohort. In A. C. Huston, & M. N. Ripke (Eds.), Developmental contexts in middle childhood: bridges to adolescence and adulthood (pp. 327–349). New York: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511499760.017.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fowles, D. C., & Kochanska, G. (2000). Temperament as a moderator of pathways to conscience in children: the contribution of electrodermal activity. Psychophysiology, 37, 788–795. doi:10.1017/S0048577200981848.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frick, P. J., Cornell, A. H., Bodin, S. D., Dane, H. E., Barry, C. T., & Loney, B. R. (2003). Callous-unemotional traits and developmental pathways to severe conduct problems. Developmental Psychology, 39, 246–260. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.39.2.246.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frick, P. J., Ray, J. V., Thornton, L. C., & Kahn, R. E. (2014). Can callous-unemotional traits enhance the understanding, diagnosis, and treatment of serious conduct problems in children and adolescents? A comprehensive review. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 1–57. doi:10.1037/a0033076.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frick, P. J., & Viding, E. (2009). Antisocial behavior from a developmental psychopathology perspective. Development and Psychopathology, 21, 1111–1131. doi:10.1017/S0954579409990071.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frick, P. J., & White, S. F. (2008). Research review: the importance of callous-unemotional traits for developmental models of aggressive and antisocial behavior. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 359–375. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01862.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hay, D. F., Payne, A., & Chadwick, A. (2004). Peer relations in childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 84–108. doi:10.1046/j.0021-9630.2003.00308.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C. (2011). Social understanding and social lives: from toddlerhood through to the transition to school. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., Dunn, J., & White, A. (1998). Trick or treat? Uneven understanding of mind and emotion and executive dysfunction in 'hard-to-manage' preschoolers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 981–994. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00401.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2006). Behavioural problems in 2-year-olds: links with individual differences in theory of mind, executive function and harsh parenting. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 488–497. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01519.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., White, A., Sharpen, J., & Dunn, J. (2000). Antisocial, angry, and unsympathetic: 'hard-to-manage' preschoolers' peer problems and possible cognitive influences. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 169–179. doi:10.1017/S0021963099005193.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, L. W., Shaw, D. S., Gardner, F., Cheong, J., Dishion, T. J., & Wilson, M. (2013). Dimensions of callousness in early childhood: links to problem behavior and family intervention effectiveness. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 347–363. doi:10.1017/S0954579412001101.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, L. W., Waller, R., & Burt, S. A. (2014). Commentary: improving treatment for youth with callous-unemotional traits through the intersection of basic and applied science—reflections on dadds et al. (2014). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55, 781–783. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12274.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, A. P., Happé, F. E., Gilbert, F., Burnett, S., & Viding, E. (2010). Feeling, caring, knowing: different types of empathy deficit in boys with psychopathic tendencies and autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 1188–1197. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02280.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kerr, D. R., Lopez, N. L., Olson, S. L., & Sameroff, A. J. (2004). Parental discipline and externalizing behavior problems in early childhood: the roles of moral regulation and child gender. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32, 369–383. doi:10.1023/B:JACP.0000030291.72775.96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kimonis, E. R., Frick, P. J., Boris, N. W., Smyke, A. T., Cornell, A. H., Farrell, J. M., & Zeanah, C. H. (2006). Callous-unemotional features, behavioral inhibition, and parenting: independent predictors of aggression in a high-risk preschool sample. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 15, 745–756. doi:10.1007/s10826-006-9047-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kimonis, E. R., Frick, P. J., Cauffman, E., Goldweber, A., & Skeem, J. (2012). Primary and secondary variants of juvenile psychopathy differ in emotional processing. Development and Psychopathology, 24(3), 1091–1103. doi:10.1017/S0954579412000557.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kimonis, E. R., Skeem, J. L., Cauffman, E., & Dmitrieva, J. (2011). Are secondary variants of juvenile psychopathy more reactively violent and less psychosocially mature than primary variants? Law and Human Behavior, 35, 381–391. doi:10.1007/s10979-010-9243-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klingzell, I., Fanti, K. A., Colins, O. F., Frogner, L., Andershed, A., & Andershed, H. (2015). Early childhood trajectories of conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits: the role of fearlessness and psychopathic personality dimensions. Child Psychiatry and Human Development. doi:10.1007/s10578-015-0560-0.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kochanska, G., & Aksan, N. (2006). Children's conscience and self-regulation. Journal of Personality, 74, 1587–1617. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00421.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kochanska, G., Gross, J. N., Lin, M., & Nichols, K. E. (2002). Guilt in young children: development, determinants, and relations with a broader system of standards. Child Development, 73, 461–482. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00418.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lahey, B. B., & Waldman, I. D. (2003). A developmental propensity model of the origins of conduct problems during childhood and adolescence. In B. B. Lahey, T. E. Moffitt, & A. Caspi (Eds.), Causes of conduct disorder and juvenile delinquency (pp. 76–117). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lemerise, E. A., & Arsenio, W. F. (2000). An integrated model of emotion processes and cognition in social information processing. Child Development, 71, 107–118. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Little, R. J., & Rubin, D. B. (2002). Statistical analysis with missing data (2nd ed., ). New York: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morrow, M. T., Hubbard, J. A., McAuliffe, M. D., Rubin, R. M., & Dearing, K. F. (2006). Childhood aggression, depressive symptoms, and peer rejection: the mediational model revisited. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30, 240–248. doi:10.1177/0165025406066757.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2004). Trajectories of physical aggression from toddlerhood to middle childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 69(4, Serial No. 278)..

  • Olson, S. L., Lopez-Duran, N., Lunkenheimer, E. S., Chang, H., & Sameroff, A. J. (2011). Individual differences in the development of early peer aggression: integrating contributions of self-regulation, theory of mind, and parenting. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 253–266. doi:10.1017/S0954579410000775.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, S. L., Sameroff, A. J., Kerr, D. C. R., Lopez, N. L., & Wellman, H. M. (2005). Developmental foundations of externalizing problems in young children: the role of effortful control. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 25–45. doi:10.1017/S0954579405050029.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O'Nions, E., Sebastian, C. L., McCrory, E., Chantiluke, K., Happé, F., & Viding, E. (2014). Neural bases of theory of mind in children with autism spectrum disorders and children with conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits. Developmental Science, 17, 786–796. doi:10.1111/desc.12167.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Pardini, D. A., Lochman, J. E., & Powell, N. (2007). The development of callous-unemotional traits and antisocial behavior in children: are there shared and/or unique predictors? Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 36, 319–333. doi:10.1080/15374410701444215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pasalich, D. S., Dadds, M. R., & Hawes, D. J. (2014). Cognitive and affective empathy in children with conduct problems: additive and interactive effects of callous–unemotional traits and autism spectrum disorders symptoms. Psychiatry Research, 219, 625–630. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2014.06.025.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Patrick, C. J., Fowles, D. C., & Krueger, R. F. (2009). Triarchic conceptualization of psychopathy: developmental origins of disinhibition, boldness, and meanness. Development and Psychopathology, 21, 913–938. doi:10.1017/S0954579409000492.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2006). Computational tools for probing interactions in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 31, 437–448. doi:10.3102/10769986031004437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruffman, T., Slade, L., & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation between children's and mothers' mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. Child Development, 73, 734–751. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00435.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, D. S., Gilliom, M., Ingoldsby, E. M., & Nagin, D. S. (2003). Trajectories leading to school-age conduct problems. Developmental Psychology, 39(2), 189–200. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.39.2.189.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shonkoff, J. P., & Phillips, D. A. (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development. Washington: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tremblay, R. E. (2000). The development of aggressive behaviour during childhood: what have we learned in the past century? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24, 129–141. doi:10.1080/016502500383232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waller, R., Gardner, F., & Hyde, L. W. (2013). What are the associations between parenting, callous–unemotional traits, and antisocial behavior in youth? A systematic review of evidence. Clinical Psychology Review, 33, 593–608. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2013.03.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waller, R., Hyde, L. W., Grabell, A. S., Alves, M. L., & Olson, S. L. (2015a). Differential associations of early callous-unemotional, oppositional, and ADHD behaviors: multiple domains within early-starting conduct problems? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry Advance Online publication. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waller, R., Wright, A. C., Shaw, D. S., Gardner, F., Dishion, T. J., Wilson, M. N., & Hyde, L. W. (2015b). Factor structure and construct validity of the parent-reported inventory of callous-unemotional traits among high-risk 9-year-olds. Assessment Advance Online publication. doi:10.1177/1073191114556101.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1989). Preschool and primary scale of intelligence– revised. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M. (2014). Making minds: How theory of mind develops. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655–684. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00304.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., Harris, P. L., Banerjee, M., & Sinclair, A. (1995). Early understanding of emotion: evidence from natural language. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 117–149. doi:10.1080/02699939508409005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Willoughby, M. T., Mills-Koonce, W. R., Gottfredson, N. C., & Wagner, N. J. (2014). Measuring callous unemotional behaviors in early childhood: factor structure and the prediction of stable aggression in middle childhood. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 36, 30–42. doi:10.1007/s10862-013-9379-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Willoughby, M. T., Waschbusch, D. A., Moore, G. A., & Propper, C. B. (2011). Using the ASEBA to screen for callous unemotional traits in early childhood: factor structure, temporal stability, and utility. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 33, 19–30. doi:10.1007/s10862-010-9195-4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by Grant #R01MH57489 from the National Institute of Mental Health to Sheryl L. Olson. We are very grateful to the children, parents, teachers, and preschool administrators who participated, and to the many individuals who helped with data collection and management. The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest. Some findings reported in full here were presented at the Society for Research in Child Development (SRCD) in Philadelphia, PA, USA in March 2015.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ju-Hyun Song.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Song, JH., Waller, R., Hyde, L.W. et al. Early Callous-Unemotional Behavior, Theory-of-Mind, and a Fearful/Inhibited Temperament Predict Externalizing Problems in Middle and Late Childhood. J Abnorm Child Psychol 44, 1205–1215 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-015-0099-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-015-0099-3

Keywords

Navigation