Skip to main content
Log in

How Effective are Instructional Explanations in Example-Based Learning? A Meta-Analytic Review

  • Review Article
  • Published:
Educational Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The worked example effect within cognitive load theory is a very well-established finding. The concrete effectiveness of worked examples in a learning situation, however, heavily depends on further moderating factors. For example, if learners improve their processing of worked examples by actively explaining the worked examples to themselves, they are usually better able to solve transfer problems. Another way to enhance example processing is to present learners with instructional explanations instead of prompting them to produce these explanations on their own. In this article, we review 21 experimental studies to address the issue whether instructional explanations support example-based learning. Meta-analytic results lead to three important conclusions: First, the benefits of instructional explanations for example-based learning per se are minimal. Second, instructional explanations are more helpful for acquiring conceptual knowledge than for acquiring procedural knowledge. Third, instructional explanations are not necessarily more effective than other methods supporting example processing such as self-explaining.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Some studies examined not only learning by studying worked examples but also by solving problems alone (e.g., Darabi et al. 2007a). The results for the experimental conditions with problem solving alone (i.e., without studying worked examples) were not included in the meta-analysis.

  2. In some studies, we inferred that participants were generally introduced into the principles to be learned from the fact that at least the topic (e.g., algebra) was introduced to them.

  3. Therefore, we excluded the study by Ward and Sweller (1990). The goal of this study (experiment 5) was to show that instructional explanations would have negative effects on example-based learning due to using a split-source format.

  4. There was one exception: In the second experiment conducted by Gerjets et al. (2006), learners in the control conditions received a combination of instructional explanations and prompts to generate self-explanations.

References

  • Anderson, J. R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89, 369–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Atkinson, R. K. (2002). Optimizing learning from examples using animated pedagogical agents. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 416–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, R. K., Derry, S. J., Renkl, A., & Wortham, D. W. (2000). Learning from examples: Instructional principles from the worked examples research. Review of Educational Research, 70, 181–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, R. K., Renkl, A., & Merrill, M. M. (2003). Transitioning from studying examples to solving problems: Combining fading with prompting fosters learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 774–783.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berthold, K., & Renkl, A. (2009). Instructional aids to support a conceptual understanding of multiple representations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 70–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berthold, K., & Renkl, A. (2010). How to foster active processing of explanations in instructional communication. Educational Psychology Review, 22, 25–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berthold, K., Eysink, T. H., & Renkl, A. (2009). Assisting self-explanation prompts are more effective than open prompts when learning with multiple representations. Instructional Science, 37, 345–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. Chichester, UK: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H., Bassok, M., Lewis, M. W., Reimann, P., & Glaser, R. (1989). Self-explanations: How students study and use examples in learning to solve problems. Cognitive Science, 13, 145–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H., de Leeuw, N., Chiu, M. H., & LaVancher, C. (1994). Eliciting self-explanations improves understanding. Cognitive Science, 18, 439–477.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biostat. (2008). Comprehensive Meta-Analysis Version 2. Englewood, NJ: Biostat (computer software).

  • *Darabi, A., Nelson, D. W., & Paas, F. (2007a). Learner involvement in instruction on a complex cognitive task: Application of a composite measure of performance and mental effort. Journal of Research on Technology in Education, 40, 39–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Darabi, A., Nelson, D. W., & Palanki, S. (2007b). Acquisition of troubleshooting skills in a computer simulation: Worked example vs. conventional problem solving instructional strategies. Computers in Human Behavior, 23, 1809–1819.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gleser, L. J., & Olkin, I. (1994). Stochastically dependent effect sizes. In H. Cooper & L. V. Hedges (Eds.), The handbook of research synthesis (pp. 339–356). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Gerjets, P., Scheiter, K., & Catrambone, R. (2003). Reducing cognitive load and fostering cognitive skill acquisition: Benefits of category-avoiding examples. In F. Schmalhofer, R. Young, & G. Katz (Eds.), Proceedings of the first European Cognitive Science Conference 2003 (pp. 450–455). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Gerjets, P., Scheiter, K., & Catrambone, R. (2006). Can learning from molar and modular worked examples be enhanced by providing instructional explanations and prompting self-explanations? Learning and Instruction, 16, 104–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Große, C. S., & Renkl, A. (2006). Effects of multiple solution methods in mathematics learning. Learning and Instruction, 16, 122–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Hausmann, R. G. M., & VanLehn, K. (2007). Explaining self-explaining: A contrast between content and generation. In R. Luckin, K. R. Koedinger, & J. Greer (Eds.), Artificial intelligence in education (pp. 417–424). Amsterdam, Netherlands: IOS Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Hilbert, T. S., Schworm, S., & Renkl, A. (2004). Learning from worked examples: The transition from instructional explanations to self-explanation prompts. In P. Gerjets, J. Elen, R. Joiner, & P. Kirschner (Eds.), Instructional design for effective and enjoyable computer-supported learning (pp. 184–192). Tübingen, Germany: Knowledge Media Research Center.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Hohn, R. L., & Moraes, I. (1997/1998). Use of rule-based elaboration of worked examples to promote the acquisition of programming plans. Journal of Computer Information Systems, 38, 35–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Hoogveld, A. W. M., Paas, F., & Jochems, W. M. G. (2005). Training higher education teachers for instructional design of competency-based education: Product-oriented versus process-oriented worked examples. Teaching and Teacher Education, 21, 287–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huedo-Medina, T. B., Sánchez-Meca, J., Botella, J., & Marín-Martínez, F. (2006). Assessing heterogeneity in meta-analysis: Q statistic or I2 index? Psychological Methods, 11, 193–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga, S., Ayres, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). The expertise reversal effect. Educational Psychologist, 38, 23–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koedinger, K. R., & Aleven, V. (2007). Exploring the assistance dilemma in experiments with cognitive tutors. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 239–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kyun, S. A., & Lee, H. (2009). The effects of worked examples in computer-based instruction: Focus on the presentation format of worked examples and prior knowledge of learners. Asia Pacific Education Review, 10, 495–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E., & Moreno, R. (2003). Nine ways to reduce cognitive load in multimedia learning. Educational Psychologist, 38, 43–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Meier, D. K., Reinhard, K. J., Carter, D. O., & Brooks, D. W. (2008). Simulations with elaborated worked example modeling: Beneficial effects on schema acquisition. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 17, 262–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Otero, J., & Graesser, A. C. (2001). PREG: Elements of a model of question asking. Cognition and Instruction, 19, 143–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pashler, H., Bain, P. M., Bottge, B. A., Graesser, A., Koedinger, K. R., McDaniel, M., et al. (2007). Organizing instruction and study to improve student learning—IES practice guide. Washington, DC: National Center for Education Research, Institute of Education Sciences, US Department of Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Reed, S. K., & Bolstad, C. A. (1991). Use of examples and procedures in problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 753–766.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Reed, S. K., Dempster, A., & Ettinger, M. (1985). Usefulness of analogous solutions for solving algebra word problems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 106–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A. (1997). Learning from worked examples: A study on individual differences. Cognitive Science, 21, 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Renkl, A. (2002). Learning from worked examples: Instructional explanations support self-explanations. Learning and Instruction, 12, 529–556.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A. (2005). The worked-example principle in multimedia learning. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 229–246). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A., Mandl, H., & Gruber, H. (1996). Inert knowledge: Analyses and remedies. Educational Psychologist, 31, 115–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rittle-Johnson, B., Siegler, R. S., & Alibali, M. W. (2001). Developing conceptual understanding and procedural skill in mathematics: An iterative process. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 346–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Schworm, S., & Renkl, A. (2006). Computer-supported example-based learning: When instructional explanations reduce self-explanations. Computers & Education, 46, 426–445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Stark, R., Gruber, H., Mandl, H., & Hinkofer, L. (2001). Wege zur Optimierung eines beispielbasierten Instruktionsansatzes: Der Einfluss multipler Perspektiven und instruktionaler Erklärungen auf den Erwerb von Handlungskompetenz [Ways to optimize an example-based instructional approach: The role of multiple perspectives and instructional explanations for the acquisition of action-based competencies]. Unterrichtswissenschaft, 29, 26–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Stark, R., Kopp, V., & Fischer, M. R. (In press). Case-based learning with worked examples in complex domains: Two experimental studies in undergraduate medical education. Learning and Instruction.

  • Sweller, J. (2005). Implications of cognitive load theory for multimedia learning. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 19–30). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., & Chandler, P. (1994). Why some material is difficult to learn. Cognition and Instruction, 12, 185–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Paas, F. G. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10, 251–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Gog, T., Paas, F., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2004). Process-oriented worked examples: Improving transfer performance through enhanced understanding. Instructional Science, 32, 83–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Van Gog, T., Paas, F., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2006). Effects of process-oriented worked examples on troubleshooting transfer performance. Learning and Instruction, 16, 154–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • *Van Gog, T., Paas, F., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2008). Effects of studying sequences of process-oriented and product-oriented worked examples on troubleshooting transfer efficiency. Learning and Instruction, 18, 211–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ward, M., & Sweller, J. (1990). Structuring effective worked examples. Cognition and Instruction, 7, 1–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wittwer, J., & Renkl, A. (2008). Why instructional explanations often do not work: A framework for understanding the effectiveness of instructional explanations. Educational Psychologist, 43, 49–64.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jörg Wittwer.

Additional information

References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Wittwer, J., Renkl, A. How Effective are Instructional Explanations in Example-Based Learning? A Meta-Analytic Review. Educ Psychol Rev 22, 393–409 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-010-9136-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-010-9136-5

Keywords

Navigation