Skip to main content
Log in

The alarm call system of wild black-fronted titi monkeys, Callicebus nigrifrons

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Upon encountering predators, many animals produce specific vocalisations that alert others and sometimes dissuade the predators from hunting. Callicebus monkeys are known for their large vocal repertoire, but little is known about the function and meaning of most call types. We recorded a large number of natural predator responses from five different groups of black-fronted titi monkeys in their Atlantic forest habitat in South Eastern Brazil. When detecting predatory threats, adult group members responded with call sequences that initially consisted of two brief, high-pitched calls with distinct frequency contours. Call A was mainly given to raptors but also to predatory capuchin monkeys and other threats within the canopy, while call B was given to predatory or non-predatory disturbances on the ground. In later parts of the sequences, we also recorded a high-pitched unmodulated call C and various low-pitched loud calls. Results therefore suggest that calls A and B provide listeners with rapid and reliable information about the general classes of danger experienced by the caller, while obtaining more specific information through other call types and combinations and behavioural responses. We discuss these findings in relation to current evolutionary theory of primate communication.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Arnold K, Zuberbühler K (2006a) The alarm-calling system of adult male putty-nosed monkeys, Cercopithecus nictitans martini. Anim Behav 72:643–653. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.11.017

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnold K, Zuberbühler K (2006b) Semantic combinations in primate calls. Nature 441:303–303. doi:10.1038/441303a

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnold K, Zuberbühler K (2008) Meaningful call combinations in a non-human primate. Curr Biol 18:R202–R203. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.01.040

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bezerra B, Barnett A, Souto A, Jones G (2009) Predation by the tayra on the common marmoset and the pale-throated three-toed sloth. J Ethol 27:91–96. doi:10.1007/s10164-008-0090-3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bianchi RDC, Mendes SL (2007) Ocelot (Leopardus pardalis) predation on primates in Caratinga Biological Station, Southeast Brazil. Am J Primatol 69:1173–1178. doi:10.1002/ajp.20415

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bicca-Marques JC, Garber PA, Azevedo-Lopes MAO (2002) Evidence of three resident adult male group members in a species of monogamous primate, the red titi monkey (Callicebus cupreus). Mammalia 66:138–142

    Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein DT (1999) Alarm calling in three species of marmots. Behaviour 136:731–757

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boinski S, Chapman CA (1995) Predation on primates: where are we and what's next? Evol Anthropol 4:1–3. doi:10.1002/evan.1360040102

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bossuyt F (2002) Natal dispersal of titi monkeys (Callicebus moloch) at Cocha Cashu, Manu National Park, Peru. Am J Phys Anthropol Supplement 34:47

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell M, Snowdon C (2007) Vocal responses of captive-reared Saguinus oedipus during mobbing. Int J Primatol 28:257–270. doi:10.1007/s10764-007-9123-y

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caro TM (2005) Antipredator defense in birds and mammals. Univ Chicago Press, Chicago, Antipredator Defense in Birds and Mammals

    Google Scholar 

  • Cäsar C, Young RJ (2008) A case of adoption in a wild group of black-fronted titi monkeys (Callicebus nigrifrons). Primates 49:146–148. doi:10.1007/s10329-007-0066-x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cäsar C, Franco ES, Soares GDN, Young RJ (2008) Observed case of maternal infanticide in a wild group of black-fronted titi monkeys (Callicebus nigrifrons). Primates 49:143–145. doi:10.1007/s10329-007-0067-9

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (1990) How monkeys see the world: Inside the mind of another species. Chicago University Press, Chicago

  • Cisneros-Heredia DF, León-Reyes A, Seger S (2005) Boa constrictor Predation on a Titi monkey, Callicebus discolor. Neotrop Primates 13:11–12. doi:10.1896/1413-4705.13.3.11

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke E, Reichard UH, Zuberbühler K (2006) The syntax and meaning of wild gibbon songs. PLoS One 1:e73

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coelho CM, De Melo LFB, Sábato MAL, Magni EMV, Hirsch A, Young RJ (2008) Habitat use by wild maned wolves (Chrysocyon brachyurus) in a transition zone environment. J Mammal 89:97–104. doi:10.1644/06-mamm-a-383.1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Luna AG, Sanmiguel R, Di Fiore A, Fernandez-Duque E (2010) Predation and predation attempts on red titi monkeys (Callicebus discolor) and equatorial sakis (Pithecia aequatorialis) in Amazonian Ecuador. Folia Primatol 81:86–95

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Derby OA (1966) The Serra of Espinhaço, Brazil. J Geol 14:374–401

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Digweed SM, Fedigan LM, Rendall D (2005) Variable specificity in the anti-predator vocalizations and behaviour of the white-faced capuchin, Cebus capucinus. Behaviour 142:997–1021

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eckardt W, Zuberbuhler K (2004) Cooperation and competition in two forest monkeys. Behav Ecol 15:400–411. doi:10.1093/beheco/arh032

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari SF (2009) Predation risk and antipredator strategies. In: Garber PA, Estrada A, Bicca-Marques JC, Heymann EW, Strier KB (eds) South American primates. Developments in primatology. Progress and Prospects, Springer New York, pp 251–277. doi:10.1007/978-0-387-78705-3_10

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fichtel C, Perry S, Gros-Louis J (2005) Alarm calls of white-faced capuchin monkeys: an acoustic analysis. Anim Behav 70:165–176. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.09.020

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freese CH, Oppenheimer JR (1981) The capuchin monkeys, genus Cebus. In: Coimbra-Filho A, Mittermeier RA (eds) Ecology and behavior of neotropical primates, vol 1. Academia Brasileira de Ciências, Rio de Janeiro, pp 331–390

    Google Scholar 

  • Giulietti AM, Pirani JR (1988) Patterns of geographic distribution of some plant species from the Espinhaço Range, Minas Gerais and Bahia, Brazil. In: Vanzolini PE, Heyer WR (eds) Proceedings of a workshop on neotropical distribution. Academia Brasileira de Ciências, Rio de Janeiro, pp 39–69

    Google Scholar 

  • Giulietti AM, Pirani JR, Harley RM (1997) Espinhaço Range region, Eastern Brazil. In: Davis SD, Heywood VH, Herrera-Macbryde O, Villa-Lobos J, Hamilton AC (eds) Centres of plant diversity, a guide and strategy for their conservation. Information Press, Oxford, pp 397–404

    Google Scholar 

  • Glantz SA, Slinker BK (2001) Primer of Applied Regression and Analysis of Variance. 2nd ed. McGraw-Hill, New York NY

  • Hirsch A (2003) Habitat fragmentation and priority areas for primate conservation in the Rio Doce Basin, Minas Gerais. Neotrop Primates 11:195–196

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinzey WG (1981) The titi monkeys, genus Callicebus. In: Coimbra-Filho AF, Mittermeier RA (eds) Ecology and behavior of neotropical primates, vol 1. Academia Brasileira de Ciências, Rio de Janeiro, pp 241–296

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinzey W, Becker M (1983) Activity pattern of the masked titi monkey, Callicebus personatus. Primates 24:337–343. doi:10.1007/bf02381979

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kinzey WG, Robinson JG (1983) Intergroup loud calls, range size, and spacing in Callicebus torquatus. Am J Phys Anthropol 60:539–544. doi:10.1002/ajpa.1330600416

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kinzey W, Rosenberger A, Heisler P, Prowse D, Trilling J (1977) A preliminary field investigation of the yellow handed titi monkey, Callicebus torquatus torquatus, in Northern Peru. Primates 18:159–181. doi:10.1007/bf02382957

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirchhof J, Hammerschmidt K (2006) Functionally referential alarm calls in tamarins (Saguinus fuscicollis and Saguinus mystax)—evidence from playback experiments. Ethology 112:346–354. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01165.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein BC, Harper LH, Bierregaard RO, Powell GVN (1988) The nesting and feeding behavior of the ornate hawk-eagle near Manaus, Brazil. Condor 90:239–245

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lawrence JM (2003) Preliminary report on the natural history of brown titi monkeys (Callicebus brunneus) at the Los Amigos Research Station, Madre de Dios, Peru. Am J Phys Anthropol Supplement 36:136

    Google Scholar 

  • Lemasson A, Ouattara K, Bouchet H, Zuberbühler K (2010) Speed of call delivery is related to context and caller identity in Campbell's monkey males. Naturwissenschaften 97:1023–1027. doi:10.1007/s00114-010-0715-6

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ludwig G, Aguiar L, Miranda J, Teixeira G, Svoboda W, Malanski L, Shiozawa M, Hilst C, Navarro I, Passos F (2007) Cougar predation on black-and-gold howlers on Mutum Island, Southern Brazil. Int J Primatol 28:39–46. doi:10.1007/s10764-006-9103-7

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macedonia JM, Evans CS (1993) Variation among mammalian alarm call systems and the problem of meaning in animal signals. Ethology 93:177–197. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1993.tb00988.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manser MB (2001) The acoustic structure of suricates' alarm calls varies with predator type and the level of response urgency. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 268:2315–2324

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Manser MB, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2002) Suricate alarm calls signal predator class and urgency. Trends Cogn Sci 6:55–57

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marler P (1955) Characteristics of some animal calls. Nature 176:6–8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin P, Bateson P (2007) Measuring behaviour: an introductory guide, 3rd edn. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Mendoza SP, Mason WA (1986) Parental division-of-labor and differentiation of attachments in a monogamous primate (Callicebus moloch). Anim Behav 34:1336–1347

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller LE, Treves A (2007) Predation on primates: past studies, current challenges, and directions for the future. In: Campbell CJ, Fuentes A, Mackinnon KC, Panger M, Bearder SK (eds) Primates in perspective. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 535–547

    Google Scholar 

  • Miranda J, Bernardi I, Moro-Rios R, Passos F (2006) Antipredator behavior of brown howlers attacked by black hawk-eagle in Southern Brazil. Int J Primatol 27:1097–1101. doi:10.1007/s10764-006-9062-z

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moynihan M (1966) Communication in the titi monkey, Callicebus. J Zool 150:77–127. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.1966.tb02999.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Müller KH (1995) Ranging in masked titi monkeys (Callicebus personatus) in Brazil. Folia Primatol 65:224–228

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Müller AE, Anzenberger G (2002) Duetting in the titi monkey Callicebus cupreus: structure, pair specificity and development of duets. Folia Primatol 73:104–115

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mundry R, Sommer C (2007) Discriminant function analysis with nonindependent data: consequences and an alternative. Anim Behav 74:965–976. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.12.028

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ouattara K, Lemasson A, Zuberbühler K (2009) Campbell’s monkeys use affixation to alter call meaning. Plos One 4:e7808. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0007808

  • Oversluijs Vasquez MR, Heymann EW (2001) Crested eagle (Morphnus guianensis) predation on infant tamarins (Saguinus mystax and Saguinus fuscicollis, Callitrichinae). Folia Primatol 72:301–303

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Owings DH, Virginia RA (1978) Alarm calls of California ground squirrels (Spermophilus beecheyi). Z Tierpsychol 46:58–70

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rendall D, Owren MJ, Ryan MJ (2009) What do animal signals mean? Anim Behav 78:233–240. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.06.007

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rendell LE, Matthews JN, Gill A, Gordon JCD, Macdonald DW (1999) Quantitative analysis of tonal calls from five odontocete species, examining interspecific and intraspecific variation. J Zool 249:403–410. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.1999.tb01209.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson JG (1979a) Analysis of the organization of vocal communication in the titi monkey Callicebus moloch. Z Tierpsychol 49:381–405

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson JG (1979b) Vocal regulation of use of space by groups of titi monkeys Callicebus moloch. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 5:1–15

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson JG (1981) Vocal regulation of inter- and intragroup spacing during boundary encounters in the titi monkey, Callicebus moloch. Primates 22:161–172. doi:10.1007/bf02382607

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson SK (1994) Habitat selection and foraging ecology of raptors in Amazonian Peru. Biotropica 26:443–458

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson JG, Wright PC, Kinzey WG (1987) Monogamous cebids and their relatives: intergroup calls and spacing. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds) In primate societies. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 44–53

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowe N (1996) The pictorial guide to the living primates. Pogonias Press, East Hampton NY

    Google Scholar 

  • Sampaio DT, Ferrari SF (2005) Predation of an infant titi monkey (Callicebus moloch) by a tufted capuchin (Cebus apella). Folia Primatol 76:113–115. doi:10.1159/000073617

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schel AM, Tranquilli S, Zuberbühler K (2009) The alarm call system of two species of black-and-white colobus monkeys (Colobus polykomos and Colobus guereza). J Comp Psychol 123:136–150. doi:10.1037/A0014280

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (1980) The ontogeny of vervet monkey alarm calling behavior: a preliminary report. Z Tierpsychol 54:37–56

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL, Marler P (1980a) Monkey responses to three different alarm calls—evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science 210:801–803

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL, Marler P (1980b) Vervet monkey alarm calls—semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. Anim Behav 28:1070–1094

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sieving KE, Hetrick SA, Avery ML (2010) The versatility of graded acoustic measures in classification of predation threats by the tufted titmouse Baeolophus bicolor: exploring a mixed framework for threat communication. Oikos 119:264–276. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0706.2009.17682.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silva JA, Talamoni SA (2003) Diet adjustments of maned wolves, Chrysocyon brachyurus (Illiger) (Mammalia, Canidae), subjected to supplemental feeding in a private natural reserve, Southeastern Brazil. Revista Brasileira de Zoologia 20:339–345

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strier KB (2007) Primate behavioral ecology. Allyn and Bacon, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS (2001) Using multivariate statistics, 4th edn. Allyn & Bacon, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  • Templeton CN, Greene E, Davis K (2005) Allometry of alarm calls: black-capped chickadees encode information about predator size. Science 308:1934–1937. doi:10.1126/science.1108841

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Terborgh J (1983) Five New World primates: a study in comparative ecology. Princeton University Press, Princeton, N.J

    Google Scholar 

  • Valeggia CR, Mendoza SP, Fernandez-Duque E, Mason WA, Lasley B (1999) Reproductive biology of female titi monkeys (Callicebus moloch) in captivity. Am J Primatol 47:183–195

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Vasconcelos MF (2001) Adições à avifauna da Serra do Caraça, Minas Gerais. Atualidades Ornitológicas. Atualidades Ornitológicas 104:3–4

    Google Scholar 

  • Vasconcelos MF, Melo Junior TA (2001) An ornithological survey of Serra do Caraça, Minas Gerais. Brazil Cotinga 15:21–31

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler B (2010) Production and perception of situationally variable alarm calls in wild tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella nigritus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 64:989–1000. doi:10.1007/s00265-010-0914-3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler K (2000a) Causal knowledge of predators' behaviour in wild Diana monkeys. Anim Behav 59:209–220

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler K (2000b) Referential labelling in Diana monkeys. Anim Behav 59:917–927

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler K (2001) Predator-specific alarm calls in Campbell's monkeys, Cercopithecus campbelli. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 50:414–422

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler K (2009) Survivor signals: the biology and psychology of animal alarm calling. Adv Stud Behav 40:277–322. doi:10.1016/S0065-3454(09)40008-1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler K, Noë R, Seyfarth RM (1997) Diana monkey long-distance calls: messages for conspecifics and predators. Anim Behav 53:589–604

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler K, Jenny D, Bshary R (1999) The predator deterrence function of primate alarm calls. Ethology 105:477–490

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank Luke Rendell and Will Hoppitt for their statistical advice and Robert Seyfarth and three other referees for their helpful comments. We are grateful to Priests Marcos, Lauro, Wilson, and Sebastião for allowing us to work in the reserve and to Aline Abreu for her support. We are thankful to Vandilso Farias for his assistance in the field and to Marina Bonde de Queiróz, Julianne Cosse de Azevedo, and Belmiro Damas for their help. This research was funded by FAPEMIG-Brazil and a doctoral dissertation scholarship by CAPES-Brazil to CC. Additional financial support was by the L.S.B. LEAKEY TRUST and the University of St Andrews. We thank John Robinson for helping with call classifications. Finally, our gratitude goes all previous members of this research project: Guedes, D., Ferreira, S.Q.C., Nahur, A.C., Assunção, M.L, Silva, P.H.N., Sena, M.L.C., Marcolino, C.P., Franco, E.S., Soares, G.C.N., Viegas, L,R.F., Afonso, C.G., Santos, R.V, Lima, B.S.S, Santos, G.P., Morais, C.M.F., Queiróz, M.B.

Ethical Standards

We received full ethical approval to conduct this study from the University of St Andrews' (School of Psychology) Ethics Committee.

Lawfulness

This work was performed in compliance with all Brazilian laws.

Conflict of interest

None of the authors have a financial relationship with the organisation that sponsored the research, i.e., FAPEMIG-Brazil and CAPES-Brazil.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Klaus Zuberbühler.

Additional information

Communicated by M. Charpentier

Appendix

Appendix

Table 6 List of type and number of potential predatory events for which the focal groups called in Caraça Private Reserve during two field seasons (May–October 2009 and May–July 2010)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Cäsar, C., Byrne, R., Young, R.J. et al. The alarm call system of wild black-fronted titi monkeys, Callicebus nigrifrons . Behav Ecol Sociobiol 66, 653–667 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-011-1313-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-011-1313-0

Keywords

Navigation