Skip to main content
Log in

The nature and nurture of expertise: a fourth dimension

  • Published:
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

One formative idea behind the workshop on expertise in Berkeley in August of 2010 was to develop a viable “trading zone” of ideas, which is defined as a location “in which communities with a deep problem of communication manage to communicate” (Collins et al. 2010, p. 8). In the current case, the goal is to have a trading zone between philosophers, sociologists, and psychologists who communicate their ideas on expertise such that productive interdisciplinary collaboration results. In this paper, I review Harry Collins' three-dimensional model of expertise and provide a very brief and non-inclusive overview of a psychological response. In addition, I propose an addendum to the model by adding a fourth—natural talent—dimension.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. I owe this distinction to an anonymous reviewer of an earlier version of this paper, who in turn was influenced by Collins.

References

  • Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (1995). Implicit gender stereotyping in judgments of fame. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 181–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bilalić, M., Kiesel, A., Pohl, C., Erb, M., & Grodd, W. (2011). It takes two-skilled recognition of objects engages lateral areas in both hemispheres. PLoS ONE, 6, e16202. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0016202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H. (2006). Two approaches to the study of experts’ characteristics. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. J. Feltovich, & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 21–30). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cianciolo, A. T., Matthew, C., Sternberg, R. J., & Wagner, R. K. (2006). Tacit knowledge, practical intelligence, and expertise. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. J. Feltovich, & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 613–632). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, H. (2011). Three dimensions of expertise. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences. doi:10.1007/s11097-011-9203-5.

  • Collins, H., Evans, R., & Gorman, M. E. (2010). Trading zones and interactional expertise. In M. E. Gorman (Ed.), Trading zones and interactional expertise: creating new kinds of collaboration (pp. 7–23). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coon, H., & Carey, G. (1989). Genetic and environmental determinants of musical ability in twins. Behavioral Genetics, 19, 183–193. doi:10.1007/BF01065903.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 25, 109–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., Charness, N., Feltovich, P. J., & Hoffman, R. R. (Eds.). (2006). The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 21–30). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feist, G. J. (2006). The psychology of science and the origins of the scientific mind. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feist, G.J. (2011). The nature and nurture of scientific talent. In S.B. Kaufman (Ed.) Innate talent vs. deliberate practice: a critical examination and future research directions. New York: Oxford University Press

  • Feist, G. J., & Gorman, M. E. (1998). Psychology of science: review and integration of a nascent discipline. Review of General Psychology, 2, 3–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feist, G. J., & Rosenberg, E. L. (2010). Psychology: making connections. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feltovich, P., Prietula, M., & Ericsson, K. (2006). Studies of expertise from psychological perspectives. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. J. Feltovich, & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 41–67). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H. (1999). Intelligence reframed: multiple intelligences for the 21st century. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J., & Light, P. (1992). Intelligence among university scientists. In R. S. Albert (Ed.), Genius and eminence (2nd ed.) (pp. 109–111). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grigorenko, E. (2000). Heritability and intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 53–91). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Helson, R., & Crutchfield, R. (1970). Mathematicians: the creative researcher and the average PhD. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 34, 250–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1979). Automatic and effortful processes in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 108, 356–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haworth, C., Wright, M., Martin, N., Martin, N., Boomsma, D., Bartels, M., et al. (2009). A twin study of the genetics of high cognitive ability selected from 11,000 twin pairs in six studies from four countries. Behavior Genetics, 39(4), 359–370. doi:10.1007/s10519-009-9262-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, N. M., & Schneider, W. (2006). Brain changes in the development of expertise: neuroanatomical and neurophysiological evidence about skill-based adaptations. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. J. Feltovich, & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 653–682). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, R. R. (1998). How can expertise be defined: implications of research from cognitive psychology. In R. Williams, W. Faulkner, & J. Fleck (Eds.), Exploring expertise (pp. 81–100). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howe, M. J., Davidson, J. W., & Sloboda, J. A. (1999). Innate talents: reality or myth? Behavorial Brain Science, 21, 399–407. doi:10.1017/S0140525X9800123X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobson, S., & Jacobson, J. (2000). Teratogenic insult and neurobehavioral function in infancy and childhood. Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: a developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lubinski, D., Benbow, C. P., Webb, R. M., & Bleske-Rechek, A. (2006). Tracking exceptional human capital over two decades. Psychological Science, 17, 194–199. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01685.x

  • Lynn, R. (2006). Race differences in intelligence: an evolutionary analysis. Augusta, GA: National Summit Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKinnon, D. W. & Hall, W. B. (1972). Intelligence and creativity. Proceedings of the XVIIth International Congress of Applied Psychology, Liege Belgium (vol. 2, pp. 1883–1888). Brussels, EDITEST.

  • Papierno, P., Ceci, S., Makel, M., & Williams, W. (2005). The nature and nurture of talent: a bioecological perspective on the ontogeny of exceptional abilities. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 28(3–4), 312–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (2002). The blank slate: the modern denial of human nature. New York: Viking.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plomin, R., & Caspi, A. (1999). Behavioral genetics and personality. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Personality: theory and research (pp. 251–276). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plomin, R., & Petrill, S. A. (1997). Genetics and intelligence: what’s new? Intelligence, 24, 53–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Polanyi, M. (1958). Personal knowledge: towards a post-critical philosophy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruthsatz, J., Detterman, D., Griscom, W., & Cirullo, B. (2008). Becoming an expert in the musical domain: it takes more than just practice. Intelligence, 36(4), 330–338. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2007.08.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. (2008). Scientific talent, training, and performance: intellect, personality, and genetic endowment. Review of General Psychology, 12(1), 28–46. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.12.1.28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Squire, L. (1987). Memory and brain. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Implicit theories of intelligence, creativity, and wisdom. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 607–627.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 381–403). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vinkhuyzen, A., van der Sluis, S., Posthuma, D., & Boomsma, D. (2009). The heritability of aptitude and exceptional talent across different domains in adolescents and young adults. Behavior Genetics, 39(4), 380–392. doi:10.1007/s10519-009-9260-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zinn, M., Zinn, M., Feist, G. J. & Schleser, R. (2006). Functional neural integration and music learning: preliminary evidence from a quantitative EEG study. Paper presented at annual conference of American Psychological Association. New Orleans, LA.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gregory J. Feist.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Feist, G.J. The nature and nurture of expertise: a fourth dimension. Phenom Cogn Sci 12, 275–288 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-011-9240-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-011-9240-0

Keywords

Navigation