Skip to main content
Log in

On the Diagnosticity of Multiple-Witness Identifications

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Law and Human Behavior

Abstract

It is not uncommon for there to be multiple eyewitnesses to a crime, each of whom is later shown a lineup. How is the probative value, or diagnosticity, of such multiple-witness identifications to be evaluated? Previous treatments have focused on the diagnosticity of a single eyewitness’s response to a lineup (Wells and Lindsay, Psychol. Bull. 3 (1980) 776); however, the results of eyewitness identification experiments indicate that the responses of multiple independent witnesses may often be inconsistent. The present paper calculates response diagnosticity for multiple witnesses and shows how diagnostic probabilities change across various combinations of consistent and inconsistent witness responses. Multiple-witness diagnosticity is examined across variation in the conditions of observation, lineup composition, and lineup presentation. In general, the diagnostic probabilities of guilt were shown to increase with the addition of suspect identifications and decrease with the addition of nonidentifications. Foil identification results were more complicated-diagnostic of innocence in many cases, but nondiagnostic or diagnostic of innocence in biased lineups. These analyses illustrate the importance of securing clear records of all witness responses, rather than myopically focusing on the witness who identified the suspect while ignoring those witnesses who did not.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. There is a potentially important distinction between cases and suspects. Yuille and Tollestrup (1992) and Tollestrup et al. (1994) reported results using cases as the unit of analysis, whereas Wright and McDaid (1996) used the suspect as the unit of analysis. Each case may involve multiple perpetrators and hence multiple suspects. The calculation of the Yuille–Tollestrup result is based on 622 cases rather than 626 shown in their Table 1 because in four homicide cases there were no witnesses. Calculations for Tollestrup et al. (1994) are based only on robbery cases, and one case that involved no witnesses is excluded for a total of 76. Calculations for Wright and McDaid are estimated from their Fig. 1 (p. 77).

  2. The calculation considers response categories rather than individual responses. Two witnesses could select a foil, but two different foils.

  3. The original treatment of diagnosticity (as developed by Wells and Lindsay 1980, and used in Wells and Turtle 1986, and in Wells and Olson 2002) used a ratio, p(R|S = G)/p(R|S ≠ G), representing the likelihood that the response would occur when the suspect was guilty versus innocent. Using the likelihood ratio index, the value of 1.0 indicates no diagnosticity, 2.0 means that the response was twice as likely if the suspect was guilty than if the suspect was innocent, and so on. Here, we have chosen to use the index p(R|S = G)/[p(R|S = G) + p(R|≠ G)], which represents diagnosticity as a probability rather than as a ratio. A probability of .50 indicates no diagnosticity, values between .50 and 1.0 indicate degrees of diagnosticity of guilt, and values between 0.0 and .50 indicate degrees of diagnosticity of innocence. The expression of diagnosticity as a probability rather than as a ratio has two advantages. First, the likelihood ratio index required the use of the obverse ratio, p(R|S ≠ G)/p(R|S = G), for responses that were diagnostic of innocence, an unnecessary step when using a probability as the index of diagnosticity. Second, the likelihood ratio is awkward (mathematically) for combining responses from multiple witnesses because it requires the multiplication of ratios (which can expand to be quite large), whereas the use of the probabilistic index of diagnosticity requires the multiplication of probabilities (which are constrained between 0 and 1).

  4. The actual distribution of identifications across lineup members is likely to vary as a function of the lineup composition. The analyses carried out here and elsewhere (Clark et al. 2007) have assumed target-absent lineups that were unbiased in their composition. This assumption arises from the fact that in the relevant studies there was no designated innocent suspect, and thus suspect identification rates in TA lineups were calculated by dividing the overall identification rate by the number of lineup members.

  5. The idea of the double-blind lineup, first proposed by Wells (1988) and emphasized strongly in lineup reform recommendations (Wells et al. 1998), would effectively eliminate the problem of the lineup administrator failing to make a clear record of foil identifications. With a double-blind lineup, the lineup administrator does not know which lineup member is the suspect and which are the foils. Hence, the record made by the double-blind administrator would have to record the identification decision with equal veracity regardless of whether it was an identification of the suspect or a foil.

References

  • Brandon, R., & Davies, C. (1973). Wrongful imprisonment. London: Allen & Unwin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, S.E., & Godfrey, R. (2007). Why eyewitnesses make mistakes and jurors believe them. Paper presented at Off the Witness Stand: Using Psychology in the Practice of Justice, New York, NY.

  • Clark, S.E., Howell, R.T., & Davey, S. (2007). Regularities in eyewitness identification. Law & Human Behavior. Retrieved May 7, 2007 from http://www.springerlink.com.

  • Clark, S. E., Hori, A., Putnam, A., & Martin, T. P. (2000). Group collaboration in recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 26, 1578–1588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, S. E. (2003). A memory and decision model for eyewitness identification. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 629–654.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, S. E. (2005). A re-examination of the effects of biased lineup instructions in eyewitness identification. Law & Human Behavior, 29, 395–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark S. E., & Tunnicliff, J. L. (2001). Selecting lineup foils in eyewitness identification experiments: experimental control and real-world simulation. Law & Human Behavior, 25, 199–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Douglass, A. B., Smith, C., & Frasher-Thill, R. (2005). A problem with double-blind photospread procedures: Photospread administrators use one eyewitness’s confidence to influence the identification of another eyewitness. Law and Human Behavior, 29, 543–562.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Findlay, K. A., & Scott, M. S. (2006). The multiple dimensions of tunnel vision in criminal cases. Wisconsin Law Review, 2006, 291–398.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gabbert, F., Memon, A., & Allan, K. (2003). Memory conformity: Can eyewitnesses influence each other’s memories for an event? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 533–543.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. (1975). Consequences of prejudice against the null hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, S. R., Jacoby, K., Matheson, D. J., Montgomery, N., & Patil, S. (2005). Exonerations in the United States 1989 through 2003. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 95, 523–560.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gross., S. R., & Thompson, J. (2005). Memo to Saul Green, Attorney for Walter Moss. Re: The Murder of Quinton Moss on June 26, 1980, in the City of St. Louis, Missouri (dated June 10, 2005).

  • Haw R. M., Fisher, R. P. (2004). Effects of administrator–witness contact on eyewitness identification accuracy. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 1106–1112.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Juslin, P., Olsson, N., Winman, A. (1996). Calibration and diagnosticity of confidence in eyewitness identification: Comments on what can be inferred from the low confidence–accuracy correlation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 22, 1304–1316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kellstrand, E. B. (2006). Eyewitness identification accuracy in cases accepted and rejected for prosecution: An archival analysis of criminal case files, Unpublished Manuscript. San Diego: University of California.

  • Krafka, C., & Penrod, S. (1985). Reinstatement of context in a field experiment on eyewitness identification. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 49, 58–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leippe, M. R. (1985). The influence of eyewitness nonidentifications on mock-jurors’ judgements of a court case. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 15, 656–672.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levi, A. M. (1998). Are defendants guilty if they were chosen in a lineup? Law and Human Behavior, 22, 389–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay, R. C. L., Martin, R., & Webber, L. (1994). Default values in eyewitness descriptions: A problem for the match-to-description lineup foil selection strategy. Law & Human Behavior, 18, 527–541.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay, R. C., & Wells, G. L. (1980). What price justice? Exploring the relationship of lineup fairness to identification accuracy. Law & Human Behavior, 4, 303–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malpass, R. S. (1981). Effective size and defendant bias in eyewitness identification lineups. Law and Human Behavior, 5, 299–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manson v. Braithwaite (1977). 432 U.S. 98.

  • McAllister, H. A., & Bregman, N. J. (1986). Juror underutilization of eyewitness nonidentifications: Theoretical and practical implications. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71, 168–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Memon, A., Hope, L., & Bull, R. (2003). Exposure duration: Effects on eyewitness accuracy and confidence. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 339–354.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, C. A., Hazlett, G., Doran, A., Garrett, S., Hoyt, G., Thomas, P., Baranoski, M., & Southwick, S. M. (2004). Accuracy of eyewitness memory for persons encountered during exposure to highly intense stress. International Journal of Law & Psychiatry, 3, 265–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neil v. Biggers (1972). 409 U.S. 188.

  • Nettles, B., Nettles, Z. S., & Wells, G. L. (1996). I noticed you paused on number three. Biased testing in eyewitness identification. Champion, Nov. pp 10–12, 57–59.

  • Nickerson, R. S. (1998). Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises. Review of General Psychology, 2, 175–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paley, B., & Geiselman, R. E. (1989). The effects of alternative photospread instructions on suspect identification performance. American Journal of Forensic Psychology, 7, 3–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paterson, H. M., & Kemp, R. I. (2006). Co-witness talk: A survey of eyewitness discussion. Psychology, Crime, & Law, 12, 181–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L, III, Meade, M. L., & Bergman, E. T. (2001). Social contagion of memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 365–371.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, G. S., & Warnick, D. H. (1981). Truth and consequences: The effect of responsibility on eyewitness behavior. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 2, 67–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, G. S., & Warnick, D. H. (1982). Evaluating identification evidence from multiple eyewitnesses. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 12, 182–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sobel, N. R. (1979). Eyewitness identification: Legal and practical problems (2nd ed.). New York: Boardman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steblay, N. M. (1997). Social influence in eyewitness recall: A meta-analytic review of lineup instruction effects. Law and Human Behavior, 21, 283–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tollestrup, P. A., Turtle, J. W., & Yuille, J. C. (1994). Actual victims and witnesses to robbery and fraud: An archival analysis. In D. F. Ross, J. D. Read, & M. P. Toglia (Eds.), Adult eyewitness testimony: Current trends and developments (pp. 144–160). New York: Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tunnicliff, J. L, & Clark, S. E. (2000). Selecting foils for identification lineups: Matching suspects or descriptions? Law & Human Behavior, 24, 231–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • U.S. v. Telfaire. (1972) 152 U.S. App. D.C. 146; 469 F.2d 552.

  • Warnick, D. H., & Sanders, G. S. (1980). Why do witnesses make so many mistakes? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 362–366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L. (1988). Eyewitness identification: A system handbook. Toronto: Carswell Legal Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., & Bradfield, A. L. (1998). “Good, you identified the suspect”: Feedback to eyewitnesses distorts their reports of the witnessing experience. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 360–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., Leippe, M. R., & Ostrom, T. M. (1979). Guidelines for empirically assessing the fairness of a lineup. Law and Human Behavior, 3, 285–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., & Lindsay, R. C. (1980). On estimating the diagnosticity of eyewitness nonidentifications. Psychological Bulletin, 3, 776–784.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., & Olson, E. A., (2002). Eyewitness identification: Information gain from incriminating and exonerating behaviors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 3, 155–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., Rydell, S. M., & Seelau, E. P. (1993). The selection of distractors for eyewitness lineups. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 835–844.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., Small, M., Penrod, S., Malpass, R. S., Fulero, S. M., & Brimacombe, C. A. E. (1998). Eyewitness identification procedures: Recommendations for lineups and photospreads. Law and Human Behavior, 22, 603–647.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., & Turtle, J. W. (1986). Eyewitness identification: The importance of lineup models. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 320–329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, D. B., Mathews, S. A., & Skagerberg, E. M. (2005). Social recognition memory: The effect of other people’s responses for previously seen and unseen items. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 11, 200–209.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, D. B., & McDaid, A. T. (1996). Comparing system and estimator variables using data from real line-ups. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 10, 75–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yuille, J. C., & Tollestrup, P. A. (1992). A model of the diverse effects of emotion on eyewitness memory. In S.-A. Christianson (Ed.) The handbook of emotion and memory: Research and theory (pp. 201–215). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Steven E. Clark.

About this article

Cite this article

Clark, S.E., Wells, G.L. On the Diagnosticity of Multiple-Witness Identifications. Law Hum Behav 32, 406–422 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-007-9115-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-007-9115-7

Keywords

Navigation