Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Random local temporal structure of category fluency responses

  • Published:
Journal of Computational Neuroscience Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The Category Fluency Test (CFT) provides a sensitive measurement of cognitive capabilities in humans related to retrieval from semantic memory. In particular, it is widely used to assess progress of cognitive impairment in patients with dementia. Previous research shows that, in the first approximation, the intensity of tested individuals’ responses within a standard 60-s test period decays exponentially with time, with faster decay rates for more cognitively impaired patients. Such decay rate can then be viewed as a global (macro) diagnostic parameter of each test. In the present paper we focus on the statistical properties of the properly de-trended time intervals between consecutive responses (inter-call times) in the Category Fluency Test. In a sense, those properties reflect the local (micro) structure of the response generation process. We find that a good approximation for the distribution of the de-trended inter-call times is provided by the Weibull Distribution, a probability distribution that appears naturally in this context as a distribution of a minimum of independent random quantities and is the standard tool in industrial reliability theory. This insight leads us to a new interpretation of the concept of “navigating a semantic space” via patient responses.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9
Fig. 10
Fig. 11
Fig. 12
Fig. 13
Fig. 14
Fig. 15

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. It would be more elegant to work here with the exponential function of the form 2 − t/τ (instead of e  − t/τ) in which case the “latency constant”, τ, would be the time by which the individual reaches 2−1 = 50% of his total capacity, but we elected not to do it as such a choice would make other calculations more messy.

  2. Even worse, as a result, the crucial de-trending process discussed in Section 3 sometimes resulted in negative inter-arrival times.

  3. The coefficient 2 in the discounting exponential factor has been chosen for mathematical convenience so that we can distribute the two factors of \(e^{t_k\nu} \) into the squared term in parentheses.

  4. All computing and graphing in this paper has been done in the symbolic manipulation language Mathematica.

  5. The idea of using the Garage Band originated with our student coauthors (JM, and TS) who administered actual CFTs for young adults YA 01–17, and older adults OA 01–17. The data for OA 01–09 were supplied by another of our coauthors (JK).

  6. Some sort of independence, perhaps fairly week, within the sequence δt 1,..., δt N , and thus, δT 1,...,δT N , is needed to justify employment of the standard statistical inference tools here. This is a delicate issue as consecutive responses are probably not completely statistically independent, and the problem is related to the global temporal structure of the category fluency tests. On the other hand, one can reasonably assume that the detrended inter-call sequences for different individuals are statistically independent.

  7. Note that the information about the total number of responses in each test is already embedded in the usual parameter N 60, we do not need it anymore.

  8. A more subtle parametric model that automatically takes into account the burstiness of the recall process will be studied in another paper.

  9. We have found recently, that a similar argument was used by Logan (1995) in the context of what he calls the “instance theory of automaticity”.

  10. This property is often called the heavy-tail property, and it means that the probabilities of big values are much larger than the corresponding probabilities for Gaussian or exponential distributions.

  11. Coincidentaly, the paper was written fifty years ago when Fred Leone was director of the Statistical Laboratory here at Case.

  12. Here we used the FindRoot facility in Mathematica.

  13. In statistics, this approach to parameteric estimation is called the Method of Moments.

References

  • Bousfield, W. A., & Sedgwick, C. H. W. (1944). An analysis of sequences of restricted associative responses. Journal of General Psychology, 30, 149–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Canning, S. J., Leach, L., Stuss, D., et al. (2004). Diagnostic utility of abbreviated fluency measures in Alzheimer disease and vascular dementia. Neurology, 24, 556–562.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caramelli, P., Carthery-Goulart, M. T., Porto, C. S., et al. (2007). Category fluency as a screening test for Alzheimer disease in illiterate and literate patients. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 21, 6–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cousineau, D., Goodman, V., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2002). Extending statistics of extremes to distributions varying on position and scale, and implication for race models. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 46, 431–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denker, N., & Woyczynski, W. A. (1998). Introductory statistics and random phenomena: Uncertainty, complexity and chaotic behavior in engineering and science. Boston: Birkhauser.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diaz, M., Sailor, K., Cheung, D., & Kuslansky, G. (2003). Category size effects in semantic and letter fluency in Alzheimer’s patients. Brain and Language, 89, 108–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellworthy, K. D. (1982). Stochastic differential equations on manifolds. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, N. (1996). Large sample theory. London: Chapman and Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gomez, R. G., & White, D. A. (2006). Using verbal fluency to detect very mild dementia of the Alzheimer type. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21, 771–775.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A., & Mandler, G. (1978). Limited processing capacity constrains the storage of unrelated sets of words and retrieval from natural categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 86–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jourdain, B., Méléard, S., & Woyczynski, W. A. (2008). Nonlinear stochastic differential equations driven by Levy processes and related partial differential equations. ALEA-Latin American Journal on Probability and Mathematical Statistics, 4, 1–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karch, G., & Woyczynski, W. A. (2007). Fractal Hamilton–Jacobi–KPZ equations. Transactions of the American Mathematical Society, 360(2007), 2423–2442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, J. H., Nelson, A., Johnson, J. K., et al. (2006). Multiple cognitive deficits in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 22, 306–311.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kwapien, S., & Woyczynski, W. A. (1992). Random series and stochastic integrals: Single and multiple. Boston: Birkhäuser.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Leone, F. C., Rutenberg, Y. H., & Topp, C. W. (1960). Order statistics and estimators for the Weibull distribution. 1960 report no. 10.26. Cleveland: Statistical Laboratory, Case Institute of Technology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, A. J., Orgrocki, P. K., & Thomas, P. J. (2009). Network graph analysis of category fluency testing. Cognitive Behavioral Neurology, 22(1), 45–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1995). The Weibull distribution, the power law, and the instance theory of automaticity. Psychological Review, 102(1.4), 751–756.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGill, W. J., & Gibbon, J. (1965). The general-gamma distribution and reaction times. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 2(1), 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, J. C., Weintraub, S., Chui, H. C., et al. (2006). The Uniform Data Set (UDS): Clinical and cognitive variables and descriptive data from Alzheimer Disease Centers. Alzheimer Disorder and Associated Disorders, 20, 210–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murdock, B. B., & Okada, R. (1970). Interresponse times in single-trial free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 86(2), 263–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piryatinska, A., Saichev, A. I., & Woyczynski, W. A. (2005). Models of anomalous diffusion: The subdiffusive case. Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and Applications, 349, 375–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pollio, H. W., Richards, S., & Lucas, R. (1969). Temporal properties of category recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 529–536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R. (1978). A theory of memory retrieval. Psychological Review, 85, 59–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R., McKoon, G., & Van Zandt, T. (1999). Connectionist and diffusion models of reaction time. Psychological Review, 106, 261–300.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R., & Murdock, Jr., B. B. (1976). Retrieval process in recognition memory. Psychological Review, 83, 190–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R., Thapar, A., & McKoon, G. (2004). A diffusion model analysis of the effects of aging on recognition memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 50(2004), 408–424.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, T., & Turvey, M. T. (2007). Human memory retrieval as Lévy foraging. Physica A, 385, 255–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rohrer, D. (1996). On the relative and absolute strength of a memory trace. Memory and Cognition, 24(2), 188–201.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rohrer, D., Wixted, J. T., Salmon, D. P., & Butters, N. (1995). Retrieval from semantic memory and its implications for Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(2.1), 1127–1139.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rouder, J. N., Tuerlinckx, F., Speckman, P., Lu, J., & Gomez, P. (2008). A hierarchical approach for fitting curves to response time measurements. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 15(6), 1201–1208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sashadri, V. (1993). The inverse Gaussian distribution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sauzeon, H., Lestage, P., Raboutet, C., et al. (2004). Verbal fluency output in children aged 7–16 as a function of the production criterion: Qualitative analysis of clustering, switching processes, and semantic network exploitation. Brain and Language, 89, 192–202.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shirani-Mehr, H., Li, C., Linag, G., & Shmueli-Scheuer, M. (2008). Quality-aware retrieval of data objects from autonomous sources for web-based repositories. http://www.ics.uci.edu/~chenli/pub/icde08-crawling.pdf.

  • Thoman, D. R., Bain, L. J., & Antle, C. E. (1969). Inferences on the parameters of the Weibull distribution. Technometrics, 11(3), 445–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tombaugh, T. N., Kozak, J., & Rees, L. (1999). Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14, 167–177.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., & Winocur, G. (1997). Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: Evidence from younger and older healthy adults. Neuropsychology, 11(1), 138–146.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Van Zandt, T. (2000). How to fit a response time distribution. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 7, 424–465.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weibull, W. (1951). A statistical distribution of wide applicability. Journal of Applied Mechanics, 18, 293–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, M. F., Neubecker, K. E., Bret, M. E., & Hynan, L. S. (2008). Language in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69(2.4), 1223–1227.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, C. N., Ratcliff, R., Vasey, M. W., & McKoon, G. (2010). Using diffusion models to understand clinical disorders. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 54, 39–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wingfield, A., & Kahana, M. J. (2002). The dynamics of memory retrieval in older adulthood. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56(3), 187–199.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wixted, J. T., & Rohrer, D. (1994). Analyzing the dynamics of free recall: An integrative review of the empirical literature. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 89–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woyczynski, W. A. (2001). Lévy processes in the physical sciences. In T. Mikosch, O. Barndorff-Nielsen, & S. Resnick (Eds.), Lévy processes—theory and applications (pp. 241–266). Boston: Birkhauser.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Woyczynski, W. A. (2005). Nonlinear partial differential equations driven by Lévy diffusions and related statistical issues. In J. Duan, & E. C. Waymire (Eds.), Probability and partial differential equations in modern applied mathematics, IMA (Vol. 140, pp. 247–258). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wu, S.-J. (2002). Estimation of the parameters of the Weibull distribution with progressively censored data. Journal of the Japan Statistical Society, 32(2), 155–163.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research has been supported by the U.S. National Science Foundation Grant “Interdisciplinary Training for Undergraduates in Biological and Mathematical Sciences” (DUE-0634612) administered by PJT; the first three authors were undergraduate students in biology and mathematics at Case while this work has been done. PJT was supported by the National Science Foundation’s program in Mathematical Biology (DMS-0720142), and acknowledges research support from the Oberlin College Libraries. JK was supported by grants from the National Institute on Aging (P30AG024978, P30AG08017 and R01AG024059).

The authors are grateful to the anonymous referees for helping us understand the broad and rich history of the studies of the distributional properties of inter-response times about which which we were initially quite ignorant partly because of the terminological diversity in the relevant literature. The paper benefited from their kind and generous advice.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Wojbor A. Woyczynski.

Additional information

Action Editor: P. Dayan

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Meyer, D.J., Messer, J., Singh, T. et al. Random local temporal structure of category fluency responses. J Comput Neurosci 32, 213–231 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10827-011-0349-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10827-011-0349-5

Keywords

Navigation