Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The oculogyral illusion: retinal and oculomotor factors

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Subjects in a dark chamber exposed to angular acceleration while viewing a head-fixed target experience motion and displacement of the target relative to their body. Competing explanations of this phenomenon, known as the oculogyral illusion, have attributed it to the suppression of the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) or to retinal slip. In the dark, the VOR evokes compensatory eye movements in the direction opposite to body acceleration. A head-fixed visual target will tend to suppress these eye movements. The VOR suppression hypothesis attributes the oculogyral illusion to the signals that prevent reflexive deviation of the eyes from the target thus resulting in apparent target displacement in the direction of acceleration. The retinal slip hypothesis attributes the illusion to inadequate fixation of the target with the eyes being involuntarily deviated in the direction opposite acceleration, the retinal slip being interpreted as target displacement in the direction of acceleration. Another possibility is that the illusion could arise from a change in the representation of the perceived head midline. To evaluate these three alternative hypotheses, we tested 8 subjects at 4 acceleration rates (2, 10, 20, 30°/s²) in each of three conditions: (a) fixate and point to a target light; (b) fixate to the target light and point to the head midline; (c) look straight ahead in the dark. The displacement magnitude of the oculogyral illusion was least at 2°/s² ≈ 2° and was ≈10° at the other acceleration rates. The presence of the target light significantly attenuated eye movements relative to the dark condition, but eye movements were still present at the 10, 20, and 30°/s² accelerations. The eye velocity profiles in the dark at different acceleration rates did not show a one-to-one inverse mapping to the magnitude of the oculogyral illusion at those rates. The perceived head midline was not significantly displaced at any of the acceleration rates. The oculogyral illusion thus has at least two contributing factors: the suppression of nystagmus at low acceleration rates and at higher acceleration rates, a partial suppression coupled with an integration of the drift of the eyes with respect to the fixation target.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Angelaki DE, Klier EM, Snyder LH (2009) A vestibular sensation: probabilistic approaches to spatial perception. Neuron 64:448–461

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Baker JT, Harper TM, Snyder LH (2003) Spatial memory following shifts of gaze. I. Saccades to memorized world-fixed and gaze-fixed targets. J Neurophysiol 89:2564–2576

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes GR (1988) Head-eye co-ordination: visual and nonvisual mechanisms of vestibulo-ocular reflex slow-phase modification. Prog Brain Res 76:319–328

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes GR, Eason RD (1988) Effects of visual and non-visual mechanisms on the vestibulo-ocular reflex during pseudo-random head movements in man. J Physiol 395:383–400

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bloomberg J, Jones GM, Segal B, McFarlane S, Soul J (1988) Vestibular-contingent voluntary saccades based on cognitive estimates of remembered vestibular information. Adv Otorhinolaryngol 41:71–75

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bloomberg J, Melvill Jones G, Segal B (1991) Adaptive plasticity in the gaze stabilizing synergy of slow and saccadic eye movements. Exp Brain Res 84:35–46

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Blouin J, Gauthier GM, van Donkelaar P, Vercher JL (1995a) Encoding the position of a flashed visual target after passive body rotations. Neuroreport 30:1165–1168

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blouin J, Gauthier GM, Vercher JL (1995b) Failure to update the egocentric representation of the visual space through labyrinthine signal. Brain Cogn 29:1–22

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Blouin J, Gauthier GM, Vercher JL (1997) Visual object localization through vestibular and neck inputs. 2: updating off-mid-sagittal-plane target positions. J Vestib Res 7:137–143

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brown RH, Imus H, Niven JI, Graybiel A (1949) The relationship between apparent displacement and motion in the oculogyral illusion. In: Mann CW, Kellum WE (eds) Research report. US Naval School of Aviation Medicine with Tulane University, Pensacola, FL, pp 1–5

  • Ceyte H, Cian C, Nougier V, Olivier I, Trousselard M (2007) Role of gravity-based information on the orientation and localization of the perceived body midline. Exp Brain Res 176:504–509

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chowdhury SA, Takahashi K, DeAngelis GC, Angelaki DE (2009) Does the middle temporal area carry vestibular signals related to self-motion? J Neurosci 29:12020–12030

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Clark B, Graybiel A (1949) The effect of angular acceleration on sound localization (The audiogyral illusion). J Psychol 28:235–244

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Clark B, Stewart JD (1969) Effects of angular acceleration on man: thresholds for the perception of rotation and the oculogyral illusion. Aerosp Med 40:952–956

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cullen KE, Rey CG, Guitton D, Galiana HL (1996) The use of system identification techniques in the analysis of oculomotor burst neuron spike traindynamic. J Comput Neurosci 3:347–368

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fluur E, Mendel L, Lagerström L (1966) Reproducibility of duration of latency time in unidirectional per-rotatory nystagmus. Acta Otolaryngol 62:118–124

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Graybiel A, Hupp DI (1946) The oculogyral illusion. A form of apparent motion which may be observed following stimulation of the semicircular canals. Aviat Med 17:3–27

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Graybiel A, Clark B, MacCorquodale K, Hupp DI (1946) Role of vestibular nystagmus in the visual perception of a moving target in the dark. Am J Psychol 59:259–266

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Graybiel A, Kerr WA, Bartley SH (1948) Stimulus thresholds of the semi-circular canals as a function of angular acceleration. Am J Psychol 61:21–36

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Guedry FE Jr (1965) Psychophysiological studies of vestibular function. Contrib Sens Physiol 14:63–135

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Israël I, André-Deshays C, Charade O, Berthoz A, Popov K, Lipshits M (1993) Gaze control in microgravity. 2. Sequences of saccades toward memorized visual targets. J Vestib Res 3:345–360

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karnath HO, Reich E, Rorden C, Fetter M, Driver J (2002) The perception of body orientation after neck-proprioceptive stimulation. Effects of time and of visual cueing. Exp Brain Res 143:350–358

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lackner JR, DiZio P (2010) Audiogravic and oculogravic illusions represent a unified spatial remapping. Exp Brain Res 202:513–518

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lackner JR, Levine MS (1979) Changes in apparent body orientation and sensory localization induced by vibration of postural muscles: vibratory myesthetic illusions. Aviat Space Environ Med 50:346–354

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lester G, Morant RB (1969) The role of felt position of the head in the audiogyral illusion. Acta Psychologica (Amst) 31:375–384

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Prieur JM, Bourdin C, Vercher JL, Sares F, Blouin J, Gauthier GM (2005) Accuracy of spatial localization depending on head posture in a perturbed gravitoinertial force field. Exp Brain Res 161:432–440

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Quarck G, Lhuisset L, Etard O, Denise P (2009) Eye eccentricity modifies the perception of whole-body rotation. Exp Brain Res 196:295–301

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schindler I, Kerkhoff G, Karnath HO, Keller I, Goldenberg G (2002) Neck muscle vibration induces lasting recovery in spatial neglect. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 73:412–419

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Seemungal BM, Gunaratne IA, Fleming IO, Gresty MA, Bronstein AM (2004) Perceptual and nystagmic thresholds of vestibular function in yaw. J Vestib Res 14:461–466

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Seizova-Cajic T, Sachtler WL (2007) Adaptation of a bimodal integration stage: visual input needed during neck muscle vibration to elicit a motion aftereffect. Exp Brain Res 181:117–129

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seizova-Cajic T, Sachtler WL, Curthoys IS (2006) Eye movements cannot explain vibration-induced visual motion and motion aftereffect. Exp Brain Res 173:141–152

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Takahashi K, Gu Y, May PJ, Newlands SD, DeAngelis GC, Angelaki DE (2007) Multimodal coding of three-dimensional rotation and translation in area MSTd: comparison of visual and vestibular selectivity. J Neurosci 27:9742–9756

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor JL, McCloskey DI (1991) Illusions of head and visual target displacement induced by vibration of neck muscles. Brain 114(Pt 2):755–759

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whiteside TCD, Graybiel A, Niven JI (1965) Visual illusions of movement. Brain 88:193–219

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Research support was provided by US Airforce Office of Scientific Research grant FA9550 06 1 0102.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jerome Carriot.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Carriot, J., Bryan, A., DiZio, P. et al. The oculogyral illusion: retinal and oculomotor factors. Exp Brain Res 209, 415–423 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2567-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2567-5

Keywords

Navigation