Skip to main content
Log in

Effects of response-set size on error-related brain activity

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

To study the effect of response-set size on action monitoring processes, the error-related negativity (Ne/ERN), the correct-related negativity (Nc/CRN), and behavioral indicators of action monitoring were compared across three groups of participants performing a two-choice, a four-choice, or an eight-choice version of the flanker task. After controlling for differential contribution of stimulus-related activity to response-locked averages resulting from large differences in response times across conditions, response-set size had strong effects on Ne/ERN and Nc/CRN. With increasing response-set size, the Ne/ERN amplitude decreased, but the Nc/CRN amplitude increased. Moreover, post-error behavioral adjustments were impaired with an increasing response-set size. These results suggest that action monitoring severely suffers when response-set size is increased. Implications of these findings for present theories of Ne/ERN and Nc/CRN are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The data from the four-choice task were already published elsewhere (Maier et al. 2008). However, the data were re-analyzed in a way that was appropriate for all three response-set size conditions. Accordingly, trimming of the behavioral data, filtering, baseline correction, and quantification of ERP components have changed.

  2. This baseline window was chosen, because it aligned the waveforms for correct and error responses with respect to the positive peaks preceding error-related brain activity. This best illustrates differences in Nc/CRN and Ne/ERN across conditions. Note, however, that the choice of baseline did not affect statistical analyses, because baseline-independent base-to-peak measures were used for component quantification.

  3. We also analyzed the data using the mean voltage in a time window of −25 to 100 ms relative to the response for the Ne/ERN and −25 to 50 ms relative to the response for the Nc/CRN. This did not change the results qualitatively.

  4. An alternative method for removing slow potentials from EEG data is to calculate so-called Surface Laplacians (see, Vidal et al. 2003).

  5. Although the error rate was the same in our three conditions, the absolute number of errors differed because of different trial numbers. To examine whether this can account for our results, we re-analyzed the data using only the first 1,280 trials from each condition. This did not change the results. Accordingly, our findings are not related to the different absolute numbers of errors in the three conditions. Another possibility is that our results reflect the different number of trials used for computing the waveforms. To control for this effect, a bootstrapping technique was applied. The smallest number of error trials in a single participant was 38. Therefore, we recomputed waveforms for each participant by randomly drawing 38 trials from each condition (correct responses and errors), and repeated this for 1000 times. For each repetition, components were quantified, and component measures were averaged across repetitions. The results obtained in this way were the same as in the main analysis, suggesting that different trial numbers were not responsible for our results.

References

  • Alegria J, Bertelson P (1970) Time uncertainty, number of alternatives and particular signal–response pair as determinants of choice reaction time. Acta Psychol 33:36–44

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein PS, Scheffers MK, Coles MG (1995) “Where did I go wrong?” A psychophysiological analysis of error detection. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21:1312–1322

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carter CS, Braver TS, Barch DM, Botvinick M, Noll D, Cohen JD (1998) Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. Science 280:747–749

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coles MGH, Scheffers MK, Holroyd CB (2001) Why is there an ERN/Ne on correct trials? Response representations, stimulus-related components, and the theory of error-processing. Biol Psychol 56:173–189

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Danielmeier C, Wessel JR, Steinhauser M, Ullsperger M (2009) Modulation of the error-related negativity by response conflict. Psychophysiology 46:1–11

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene S, Posner MI, Tucker DM (1994) Localization of a neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychol Sci 5:303–305

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delorme A, Makeig S (2004) EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods 134:9–21

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Delorme A, Sejnowski T, Makeig S (2007) Enhanced detection of artifacts in EEG data using higher-order statistics and independent component analysis. Neuroimage 34:1443–1449

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen BA, Eriksen CW (1974) Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept Psychophys 16:143–149

    Google Scholar 

  • Falkenstein M, Hohnsbein J, Hoormann J, Blanke L (1990) Effects of errors in choice reaction tasks on the ERP under focused and divided attention. In: Brunia CHM, Gaillard AWK, Kok A (eds) Psychophysiological brain research, 1. University Press, Tilburg, pp 192–195

    Google Scholar 

  • Falkenstein M, Hoormann J, Christ S, Hohnsbein J (2000) ERP components on reaction errors and their functional significance: a tutorial. Biol Psychol 51:87–107

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gehring WJ, Goss B, Coles MGH, Meyer DE, Donchin E (1993) A neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychol Sci 4:385–390

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gratton G, Coles MGH, Donchin E (1983) A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 55:468–484

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hajcak G, Vidal F, Simons RF (2004) Difficulties with easy tasks: ERN/Ne and stimulus component overlap. In: Ullsperger M, Falkenstein M (eds) Errors, conflicts, the brain. Current opinions on performance monitoring, 1. Max Planck Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, Leipzig, pp 204–211

    Google Scholar 

  • Hajcak G, Moser JS, Yeung N, Simons RF (2005) On the ERN and the significance of errors. Psychophysiology 42:151–160

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hick WE (1952) On the rate of gain of information. Q J Exp Psychol 4:11–26

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holroyd CB, Coles MGH (2002) The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychol Rev 109:679–709

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laming D (1979) Choice reaction performance following an error. Acta Psychol 43:199–224

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logan GD (1988) Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychol Rev 95:492–527

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luu P, Tucker DM (2001) Regulating action: alternating activation of midline frontal and motor cortical networks. Clin Neurophysiol 112:1295–1306

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luu P, Flaisch T, Tucker DM (2000) Medial frontal cortex in action monitoring. J Neurosci 20:464–469

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maier M, Steinhauser M, Hübner R (2008) Is the error-related negativity amplitude related to error detectability? Evidence of effects from different error types. J Cogn Neurosci 20:2263–2273

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pailing PE, Segalowitz SJ (2004) The effects of uncertainty in error monitoring on associated ERPs. Brain Cogn 56:215–233

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rabbitt PM (1966) Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. J Exp Psychol 71:264–272

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rabbitt PMA (1967) Time to detect errors as a function of factors affecting choice-response time. Acta Psychol 27:131–142

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rabbitt PMA (2002) Consciousness is slower than you think. Q J Exp Psychol Hum Exp Psychol 55:1081–1092

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheffers MK, Coles MGH (2000) Performance monitoring in a confusing world: error-related brain activity, judgments of response accuracy and types of errors. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 26:141–151

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steinhauser M, Maier M, Hübner R (2008) Modeling behavioral measures of error detection in choice tasks: response monitoring versus conflict monitoring. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 34:158–176

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ullsperger M, von Cramon D (2001) Subprocesses of performance monitoring: a dissociation of error processing and response competition revealed by event-related fMRI and ERPs. Neuroimage 14:1387–1401

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Usher M, Olami Z, McClelland JL (2002) Hick’s law in a stochastic race model with speed–accuracy tradeoff. J Math Psychol 46:704–715

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vidal F, Hasbroucq T, Grapperon J, Bonnet M (2000) Is the ‘error negativity’ specific to errors? Biol Psychol 51:109–128

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vidal F, Burle B, Bonnet M, Grapperon J, Hasbroucq T (2003) Error negativity on correct trials: a reexamination of available data. Biol Psychol 64:265–282

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yeung N, Botvinick MM, Cohen JD (2004) The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. Psychol Rev 111:931–959

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Martin E. Maier.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Maier, M.E., Steinhauser, M. & Hübner, R. Effects of response-set size on error-related brain activity. Exp Brain Res 202, 571–581 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-010-2160-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-010-2160-3

Keywords

Navigation