Skip to main content
Log in

Dream Characters’ Types of Involvement and Range of Relational Capacities in Non-Lucid Problem-Solving Dreams (Part 2)

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Activitas Nervosa Superior

Abstract

Dream characters continue their enduring presence in dreams in general as the prevalent feature of dreaming consciousness. The present work focuses on non-lucid problem-solving dreams, and dream characters’ types of involvement and range of their relational capacities. The analysis of 979 cross-cultural problem-solving dreams using the method of grounded theory yielded 56 exemplary dreams. From the perspective of cross-state consciousness, the tentative explanatory frameworks include attained waking-life relational schemas, habitual or emerging patterns of organizing inner experiences in distress, and potential reliance on adaptive involuntary homeostatic mechanisms. The discussion of results centers on dream characters’ positive and negative involvements in dreamers’ portrayals of family constellations, and dreamers’ reactions and responses to dream characters’ experiencing problems separately from the dreamer and together with the dreamer. In terms of relational capacities, dream characters display beneficence toward the dreamer in the form of kindness, helpful intentions, and behaviors, or they instigate problems for the dreamers and display injurious and harmful attitudes, intentions, or behaviors. Limitations of the study include a study sample from highly motivated dreamers. Further research might focus on similarities or differences in dream characters’ involvement in problematic situations dreamers experience during lucid dreaming.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Italicized, by the present author, for emphasis in relationship to current subject matter.

  2. As I indicated previously (e. g., Kozmová 2008, 2017c), because the experiences in the dreams are predominantly active and the analysis of the dreams is also an active process (e.g., dreamers think, act, and dream characters behave, etc.), the author presents the dream characters’ activities in the vivid narrative present tense. This type of description is in contrast to reports of results in the past tense.

References

  • Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1991). Attachment and other affectional bonds across the life cycle. In C. M. Parkes, J. Stevenson-Hinde, & P. Morris (Eds.), Attachment across the life cycle (pp. 33–51). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, D. (1995). The dream characters as prototype for multiple personality alter. Dissociation, 53(1), 61–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, D., & McNamara, P. (Eds.). (2007). The new science of dreaming. Vol. 1, 2, 3. Westport: Praeger.

  • Blagrove, M. (1992). Scripts and structuralist analysis of dreams. Dreaming, 2(1), 23–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. London, England, Hogarth Press.

  • Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss. Vol. 2. Separation. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss. Vol. 3. Loss, sadness, and depression. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brenneis, C. B., & Roll, S. (2009). Ego modalities in the manifest dreams of male and female Chicanos. Psychiatry, 38, 172–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Domhoff, G. W. (1996). Finding meaning in dreams: a quantitative approach. New York: Plenum.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Domhoff, G. W. (2003). Scientific study of dreams: neural networks, cognitive development, and content analysis. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity: youth and crisis. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiss, H. (1993). The “Royal Road” to the unconscious revisited: A signal detection model of dream function. In A. Moffitt, M. Kramer, & R. Hoffmann (Eds.), The functions of dreaming (pp. 381–418). New York: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (2009). Theoretical models of psychodynamic psychotherapy. In G. O. Gabbard (Ed.), Textbook of psychotherapeutic treatments (pp. 3–42). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1912). The dynamics of transference. (J. Strachey, trans.). In J. Strachey (Ed.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 12, pp. 99–108). London: Hogarth Press.

  • Freud, S. (1933). Revision of the theory of dreams. The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud, volume XX (1932–1936): new introductory lecture on psychoanalysis and other works, (pp. 6–29). London: The Hogarth Press and the Institute of Psycho-analysis.

  • Gabbard, G. O. (2004). Long-term psychodynamic psychotherapy: a basic text. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glucksman, M. L. (2007). Dreaming: an opportunity for change. Plymouth: Jason Aronson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, A. (Ed.). (1980). Advances in self-psychology. With summarizing reflections by Heinz Kohut. New York: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, C. S., & Van de Castle, R. L. (1966). The content analysis of dreams. New York: Appleton-Century-Croft.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henwood, K., & Pidgeon, N. (2003). Grounded theory in psychological research. In P. M. Camic, J. M. Rhodes, & L. Yardley (Eds.), Qualitative research in psychology: expanding perspectives in methodology and design (pp. 131–155). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Glaser, B. G., & Strauss, A. L. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory: strategies for qualitative research. Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, R. (1987). On the importance of wearing two hats: dream researcher and psychoanalyst. Journal of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis and Dynamic Psychiatry, 15(3), 321–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hollan, D. (2004). An anthropology of dreaming: selfscape dreams. Dreaming, 14, 170–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, J. (1979). Attachment, autonomy, intimacy. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 70, 231–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jung, C. G. (1974). Dreams. Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press.

  • Knox, J. (2011). Self-agency in psychotherapy: attachment, autonomy, and intimacy. New York: W. W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohut, H., & Wolf, E. S. (1978). The disorders of the self and their treatment. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 59, 413–425.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kozmová, M. (2008). The investigation of nocturnal cognitive problem-solving using cross-cultural dreams. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Saybrook University, San Francisco, CA.

  • Kozmová, M. (2012). Dreamers as agents making strategizing efforts exemplify core aggregate of executive function in non-lucid dreaming. International Journal of Dream Research, 5(1), 47–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kozmová, M. (2015). Emotions during non-lucid problem-solving dreams as evidence of secondary consciousness. Comp Psychol, 4, 6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kozmová, M. (2017a). Review of varieties of dream characters’ presence in non-lucid dreams. Activitas Nervosa Superior, 59(2), 37–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kozmová, M. (2017b). Non-lucid dreamers actualize volition as ego executive capacity by engaging in problem-solving. International Journal of Dream Research, 10(1), 30–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kozmová, M. (2017c). Dream characters’ types of involvement and range of relational capacities in non-lucid problem-solving dreams. Activitas Nervosa Superior, 59(2), 91–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kozmová, M., & Wolman, R. N. (2006). Self-awareness in dreaming. Dreaming, 16(3), 196–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krippner, S., & Weinhold, J. (2001). Gender differences in content analysis of 240 dream reports from Brazilian participants in dream seminars. Dreaming, 11(1), 35–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, T. (2011). Dream analysis and classification: The reality simulation perspective. Chapter 51. In M. H. Kryger, T. Roth, & W. C. Dement (Eds.), Principles and practice of sleep medicine (5th ed., pp. 595–603). S. Louis: Elsevier Saunders.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Perls, F. (1969). Gestalt therapy verbatim. Moab: Real People Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Purcell, S., Moffitt, A., & Hoffmann, R. (1993). Waking, dreaming, and self-regulation. In A. Moffitt, M. Kramer, & R. Hoffmann (Eds.), The functions of dreaming (pp. 197–260). Albany: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rechtschaffen, A. (1978). The single-mindedness and isolation of dreams. Sleep, 1, 97–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Revonsuo, A., Touminen, J., & Valli, K. (2015). The avatars in the machine: Dreaming as a simulation of social reality. In T. W. Metzinger & J. (Eds.), Open MIND: philosophy and the mind sciences in 21 st century (Vol. 2, pp. 1295–1322). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silberer, H. (1951). Report on a method of eliciting and observing certain symbolic hallucination-phenomena. In Organization and pathology of thought: selected sources. (D. Rappaport, trans.; pp. 195–207). New York: Columbia University Press.

  • Spruiell, V. (1983). The rules and frames of the psychoanalytic situation. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 52, 1–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of infants: A view from psychoanalysis and developmental psychology. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. N., Sander, L. W., Nahum, J. P., Harrison, A. M., Lyons-Ruth, K., Morgan, A. C., Bruschweiller-Stern, N., et al. (1998). Non-interpretive mechanisms in psychoanalytic therapy: the ‘something more’ than interpretation. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 79, 903–921.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strauch, I., & Meier, B. (1996). In search of dreams: results of experimental dream research. Albany: State University of New York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stumbrys, T., Erlacher, D., & Schmidt, S. (2011). Lucid dram mathematics: an explorative online study of arithmetic abilities of dream characters. International Journal of Dream Research, 4(10), 35–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tholey, P. (1989). Consciousness and abilities of dream characters observed during lucid dreaming. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 68(2), 567–578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Emotions and emotional communication in infants. American Psychologist, 44, 112–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaillant, G. (1995). Loss as a metaphor for attachment. American Journal of Psychoanalysis, 45(1), 59–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaillant, G. E. (2011). Involuntary coping mechanisms: a psychodynamic perspective. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 13(3), 366–369.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Van Eeden, F. (1913). A study of dreams. Proceedings of the Society for Psychical Research, 26, 413–461.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vaughan, A. G. (2016). Jungian dreamwork. In J. E. Lewis & S. Krippner (Eds.), Working with dreams and PTSD nightmares (pp. 1–21). Santa Barbara: Praeger, ABC-CLIO.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Franz, M.-L. (1998). Dreams: a study of dreams of Jung, Descartes, Socrates, and the other historical figures. Boston: Shambhala Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waldinger, R. J., Seidman, E. L., Gerber, A. J., Liem, J. H., Allen, J. P., & Hauser, S. T. (2003). Attachment and core relationship themes: wishes for autonomy and closeness in the narratives of securely and insecurely attached adults. Psychotherapy Research, 13(1), 77–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolman, R. N., & Kozmová, M. (2007). Last night I had the strangest dream: varieties of rational thought processes in dream reports. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(4), 838–849.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Miloslava Kozmová.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that she has no conflict of interest.

Additional information

The precursor of the present article entitled “Review of Varieties of Dream Characters’ Presence in Non-Lucid Dreams” (Kozmová 2017a) was published in Activitas Nervosa Superior, 59(2), 37–42, and it preceded the present research. The author presented the results of the present research at the conference titled Harvard Medical School Psychiatry Research Day, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts, April 12, 2017. The subsequent paper entitled “Dream Characters’ Types of Involvement and Range of Relational Capacities in Non-Lucid Problem-Solving Dreams, Part 1” was published in Activitas Nervosa Superior, 59(3), 91–105. The current paper continues with a discussion of results, limitations of study, and suggestions for further research.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kozmová, M. Dream Characters’ Types of Involvement and Range of Relational Capacities in Non-Lucid Problem-Solving Dreams (Part 2). Act Nerv Super 60, 48–58 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s41470-018-0020-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s41470-018-0020-9

Keywords

Navigation