Introduction

The transmission of communicable disease has become a pervasive threat as human populations grow. Densely populated ecologies are prone to infectious disease through increased interpersonal contact (Jones et al., 2008; Rocklöv & Sjödin, 2020). Increasing urbanization has led to a greater likelihood of individuals residing in densely populated areas (Bloom et al., 2008). This urbanization naturally increases experiences of crowding that correspondingly increase risk of pathogenic illness exposure. This risk became particularly salient during the COVID-19 pandemic, which saw the employment of extensive social distancing guidelines that reduced the requisite contact for disease spread and a considerable migration from individuals away from urban environments (Gagnon et al., 2020; Whitaker, 2021). The resulting vigilance toward pathogenic threat from interpersonal contact would similarly elicit motivations and behaviors to ensure individuals the opportunity to maintain access to benefits of group living while minimizing risk of disease spread.

In addition to disease salience eliciting perceptions of crowded spaces as more threatening (Wang & Ackerman, 2019), crowd salience elicits perceptions of oneself as vulnerable to disease and motivates interpersonal reticence to reduce the fitness costs of infection (Brown & Sacco, 2019; Maeng, Tanner, & Soman, 2013; Sng et al., 2017). This caution may further elicit consideration of which individuals optimize group living in densely populated environments. It could prove advantageous to identify group members whose appearance suggests reduced risk of disease transmission through healthy facial features. Crowd salience could heighten this sensitivity toward these features given the inevitability of interpersonal contact that would warrant identification of likely threats preemptively to prevent contact with diseased conspecifics (Neuberg, Kenrick, & Schaller, 2011). This study sought to identify how crowd salience activates pathogen-avoidant motives as indexed by subsequent interpersonal preferences based on virulence cues.

Crowding and Threat Management

Despite myriad benefits of group living, human sociality remains bounded, and oversaturation is possible. One route through which the oversaturation could emerge is the feeling of crowding. Crowding is the psychological discomfort from being in environments in which there are more people occupying a given space than can be comfortably accommodated (Cain & LeDoux, 2008). Crowding creates a unique experience wherein the risks of exposure to (many) other people far outweigh any benefits. Perceptions of oneself as feeling crowded subsequently elicits feelings of constriction (Gochman & Keating, 1980; Stockdale, 1978). Such feelings foster motivations for social withdrawal (Baum & Valins, 1979), heightened anxiety (Schaeffer & Patterson, 1980), and sympathetic nervous system responses (Aiello et al., 1977; Kennedy et al., 2009).

Subsumed within this social withdrawal are myriad cautious responses to minimize safety and health threats to posed by these environments. Individuals in densely populated ecologies typically express greater disinterest in promiscuous mating strategies, a behavioral repertoire that is frequently associated with greater disease transmission (Sng et al., 2017). Recent findings suggest that population density is further associated with reduced fertility rates across 174 nations, a response that could reflect a reduction of potential disease hosts in an already saturated ecology (Rotella et al., 2020). Temporally activating concerns of a crowd further leads to greater interest in consuming various emergency materials serving to reduce risk of illness and injury (Maeng et al., 2013). These findings indicate crowd salience downregulates interest in behaviors that could increase the likelihood of incurring health costs, suggesting their overall salience could heighten aversion to threats beyond those related to physical safety.

Behavioral Components of the Immune System and Social Behavior

To combat pathogenic threats, organisms possess a sophisticated immunological response system comprised of innate (e.g., cellular barriers) and adaptive subsystems (antigenic responses) that provide a multilayered defense against disease vectors (Litman, Cannon, & Dishaw, 2005; Pancer & Cooper, 2006). However, many of their more salient responses toward infections remain metabolically costly (e.g., white blood cells, fevers), with substantial caloric expenditures (i.e., 13% increase in metabolic activity) to raise one’s body temperature by 1 °C (Kluger, 1991). These immunological responses could thus divert resources away from physiological systems involved in other fitness-enhancing behaviors (e.g., eating, mating). As a complement to these responses, research suggests organisms evolved a concomitant behavioral component of the immune system involved in the identification, and avoidance, of disease vectors preemptively to reduce the likelihood of contracting infections through physical contact to reduce the necessity of relying entirely on physiological responses (Murray & Schaller, 2016). Behavioral responses to disease cues are common in various species, including perceptual acuity toward diseased conspecifics in rodents (Kavaliers, Choleris, & Pfaff, 2005), ostracism of infected group members among chimpanzees (Goodall, 1986), and proactive lining of nests with antibiotic resin in ants (Chapuisat et al., 2007).

A growing preponderance of evidence has emerged demonstrating similar responses among humans. Responses include affective disgust toward food with a heightened likelihood of parasite contamination (Al-Shawaf et al., 2015) and specific behaviors designed to remove ectoparasites in the presence of social cues to disease (e.g., contagious itching; Kupfer & Fessler, 2018). Because of the considerable risk of disease transmission through interpersonal contact (Jones et al., 2008), various interpersonal behaviors have been implicated as responses to this possibility across cultures. Geopolitical regions experiencing greater prevalence of infectious disease frequently see the emergence of cultures that valuate interpersonal reticence and restricted sexual strategies (Schaller & Murray, 2008). These restrictive behaviors further include adoption of collectivistic values in regions with heightened pathogen loads (Fincher et al., 2008), greater valuation of conformity within the corresponding countries (Murray, Trudeau, & Schaller, 2011; Wu & Chang, 2012), and interest in authoritarian governance (Murray, Schaller, & Suedfeld, 2013). Both chronic and acute concerns of disease further foster interpersonal reticence (Brown & Sacco, 2016; Mortensen et al., 2010; Tybur et al., 2020), aversion to physical contact with others (Brown & Sacco, 2020; Brown, Young, & Sacco, 2021; Makhanova & Shepherd, 2020; Sawada, Auger, & Lydon, 2018), and downregulated affiliative motives to reduce the physical contact necessary for disease transmission (Sacco, Young, & Hugenberg, 2014).

Aversive responses to pathogenic threats include the identification of potential disease risk among conspecifics, leading to greater aversion toward those possessing putative cues toward disease. These aversive responses operate within an overgeneralization principle, wherein humans perceive an overabundance of social stimuli as infectious to minimize costly failures in classifying actual threats (Haselton & Nettle, 2006). Liberal identification criteria foster withdrawal from those whose appearance heuristically connotes disease threat, even if such appearance is not indicative of an actual infection risk (e.g., obesity; Miller & Maner, 2012). Such responses include vigilance toward disfigurements (Ackerman et al., 2009), aversion to non-normative and unfamiliar conspecifics (Murray & Schaller, 2012; Peng, Chang, & Zhou, 2013), and a heightened desire to penalize others (Brown et al., 2017).

Interplays Between Disease Avoidance and Crowds

Despite the risk of infectious disease in group living, humans are nonetheless willing to incur the potential costs provided they do not exceed the benefits afforded through group living. Exclusionary experiences specifically reduce concerns about contracting infections and heighten individuals’ desire to affiliate with those who present increased risk of disease transmission through extensive interpersonal contact (i.e., extraverts; Brown et al., 2019a, 2019b; Sacco et al., 2014). However, the potential benefits of group living remain bounded as individuals could experience an oversaturation in interpersonal contact and begin to recognize the costs as exceeding any potential benefits (Tybur & Lieberman, 2016).

Densely populated ecologies experience increased likelihood of disease transmission (Hoang et al., 2019; Salathé et al., 2010), which could result in perceptions of socializing in crowds as being higher in costs (i.e., disease transmission) than benefits (i.e., affiliation opportunities). Disease salience amplifies perceptions of crowds as disease vectors (Wang & Ackerman, 2019), amplifying desires to reduce contact with disease cues. Given this demonstrable interplay between interpersonal contact and disease transmission, recent research has provided further evidence for an implicit understanding of crowds as pathogenically threatening. After rendering a crowding experience salient, individuals reported heightened vigilance toward pathogenic threats (Brown & Sacco, 2019). This salience further shifted interpersonal preferences by heightening preferences for facial symmetry, a feature associated with reduced pathogen load (Thornhill & Gangestad, 2006).

Current Research

This study sought to replicate and extend previous findings demonstrating how crowd salience heightens aversion to disease cues. Crowding indeed heightens preferences for facial features putatively associated with health, particularly through facial symmetry (Brown & Sacco, 2019). However, this preference for symmetry leaves it unclear whether this preference is rooted in an interest in healthy conspecifics or an aversion to those who appear diseased (see Zebrowitz & Rhodes, 2004), given the specific signal value of symmetry and nature of the preference task in previous work. To reconcile this ambiguity, we specifically considered facial features that would be specifically diagnostic of infection risk instead of reduced pathogen load like with symmetry. Participants were specifically tasked with evaluating target individuals exhibiting skin lesions that are heuristically associated with poor health and foster aversive behaviors among those motivated to avoid disease (van Leeuwen & Petersen, 2018). This evaluative task further afforded us the opportunity to consider both interest and aversion to associating with targets as a function of their health status directly.

This methodological decision to focus on perceptions of disease cues further afforded us the opportunity to have a comparison with faces that appeared relatively healthy from which we could consider complementary hypotheses related to individuals’ motivation to avoid disease. Specifically, we could assess whether evaluations of targets could reflect a preference for healthy facial features in an ostensibly pathogenic environment (Ainsworth & Maner, 2019; Young, Sacco, & Hugenberg, 2011), or an aversion to facial features connoting poor health (e.g., Ackerman et al., 2009). If crowding-induced preferences for health cues are rooted in preferences to associate with healthy conspecifics, crowd salience should upregulate interest in healthy conspecifics. Conversely, if this preference is rooted in responses to avoid diseased conspecifics, crowd salience should heighten aversion to disease cues. Data and materials are available through the following link: https://osf.io/n8u6m/.

Methods

Participants

We recruited 239 participants from two universities for course credit. One was a public university in a Southern U.S. state (n = 144) and the other a private university in a Northeastern U.S. state (n = 95) from February to May 2020. No data were excluded from final analyses (190 women, 49 men; MAge = 20.43, SD = 3.05; 52% White). We collected data at both sites in the service of increasing statistical power in an online study due to constraints imposed by the COVID-19 pandemic that limited data collection opportunities and necessitated use of two sites. We had no a priori predictions for potential differences between universities and did not analyze data until we finished data collection; our stop rule was to collect as many participants as possible until the end of the semester. Sensitivity analyses indicated adequate power for small effects to test for the basic predicted interactive effects for a 2 (Condition: Crowding vs. Control) × 2 (Target Status: Diseased vs. Healthy) mixed experimental design (f = 0.09, 1 − β = 0.80).

Materials and Procedure

Crowd Salience

Participants were primed by envisioning themselves in one of two public spaces as means to render salient the feelings of being in crowd (Maeng et al., 2013). One space was a large mass of people in a crowded environment (n = 121), and the other a few individuals spread far apart as a control condition (n = 118). Participants described their hypothetical feelings in either image for a 5-min writing task in a paradigm that has previously demonstrated efficacy in eliciting the salience of a crowd while also being distinct from other negative motivational states (e.g., loneliness, hunger; Brown & Sacco, 2019).Footnote 1 Participants responded to a single-item general affect item on a 7-point scale (− 3 = Extremely Negative; + 3 = Extremely Positive), and 6-item manipulation check assessing feelings of crowding (1 = Not at All; 7 = Very Much; α = 0.95).

Target Faces

Participants evaluated four Caucasian target men, manipulated to present physical cues connoting either health or disease (van Leeuwen & Petersen, 2018). We presented two unique identities whose faces appeared healthy and two unique identities altered to present physical cues indicating infectious disease. Two versions of each face existed; we counterbalanced which version of was presented to ensure participants evaluated two faces for each category to prevent stimulus effects that were further presented in a randomized order.

Accompanying each target were two questions assessing comfort with proximity to each target in general affiliative domains on 11-point scales (− 5 = Very Uncomfortable; 0 = Neutral; 5 = Very Comfortable). We aggregated scores for both items in each target category separately and found items for both healthy and diseased faces highly correlated (rs > 0.81, ps < 0.001). These high correlations prompted us to consider these variables as a singular item for both categories.

Consenting participants were randomly assigned to one of the two priming conditions and responded to the manipulation check. Participants then evaluated target faces before providing demographics and debriefing.Footnote 2

Results

Preliminary Analyses

An independent samples t-test indicated crowding-primed participants reported feeling more crowded (M = 5.07, SD = 1.42) than control-primed participants (M = 2.03, SD = 1.09), t(224.71) = 18.48, p < 0.001, d = 2.38. Crowding-primed participants additionally reported more negative affect (M =  − 0.19, SD = 0.67) than control-primed participants (M = 0.67, SD = 1.21), t(237) =  − 4.85, p < 0.001, d =  − 0.62. Crowding-primed participants’ greater negative affect prompted us to consider whether results were influenced by feelings of crowding or affect. This possibility led us to include affect as a covariate in this model to determine the unique role of crowd salience independent of the changes in affect that could influence interpersonal decision-making (see Brown & Sacco, 2020).Footnote 3

Primary Analyses

We entered participants’ collapsed affiliation responses into a 2 (Condition: Crowding = 1, Control =  − 1) × 2 (Region: South = 1, North =  − 1) × 2 (Target Status: Diseased = 1, Healthy =  − 1) linear mixed effects model that included random intercepts of participants and target stimuli, a correlated random slope of Target Status for participants and an uncorrelated random slope of Target Status for stimuli, and nested participants in Region. Two-level factors were contrast-coded as is recommended when using such models (see Judd, McClelland, & Ryan, 2017).Footnote 4 Our decision to collect data at two different author-affiliated universities in separate U.S. regions led us to use the region as a between-subjects factor to identify possible differences in responses on an exploratory basis. Overall model fit indices indicate that a moderate amount of variance is accounted for by both the fixed and random effects, R2marginal = .193, ICCconditional = .629.Footnote 5 Data are summarized in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Average willingness to have contact with diseased versus healthy targets for Southern (left panel) and Northern (right panel) participants, separated by crowded versus control condition. Error bars represent 95% CIs of the means

A Target Status main effect emerged indicating that participants showed less aversion toward contact with a healthy target (M =  − 0.48, SD = 2.34) than toward a diseased target (M =  − 2.36, SD = 2.41), b =  − .91, SE = .09, t(3.60) =  − 8.43, p = .002, β =  − .37, 95% CIβ [− .45, − .29], ηp2 = .95. A main effect of Region also emerged indicating Southern participants were less likely to have contact with any individual (M =  − 1.78, SD = 2.50) than Northern participants (M =  − .87, SD = 2.55), b =  − .47, SE = .13, t(234.03) =  − 3.64, p < .001, β =  − .18, 95% CIβ [− .28, − .08], ηp2 = .05. The Condition main effect was additionally not significant, b = .23, SE = .13, t(234.32) = 1.69, p = .09, β = .07, 95% CIβ [− .03, .17], ηp2 = .01. A main effect of Affect as the covariate was significant, b = .19, SE = .09, t(233.99) = 2.06, p = .04, β = .11, 95% CIβ [.01, .21], ηp2 = .02.

Predicted Interaction

These main effects were initially qualified by a significant Target Status × Condition interaction, b =  − .15, SE = .05, t(234.99) =  − 2.24, p = .02, β =  − .05, 95% CIβ [− .10, .00], ηp2 = .02. We decomposed this interaction, while including the Affect covariate in each model, to explore the effect of Target Status for each Condition separately. For control-primed participants, we found a significant effect of Target Status, such that participants evinced a much lower desire to contact a diseased (M =  − 2.33, SD = 2.24) versus a healthy target (M =  − .70, SD = 2.20), b =  − .81, SE = .11, t(7.29) =  − 7.29, p < .001, β =  − .35, 95% CIβ [− .44, − .25], ηp2 = .98. For crowding-primed participants, there was again a significant effect of Target Status, wherein participants again showed less desire to contact a diseased (M =  − 2.39, SD = 2.57) than a healthy target (M =  − .26, SD = 2.46), b =  − 1.07, SE = .13, t(4.47) =  − 8.16, p < .001, β =  − .39, 95% CIβ [− .49, − .30], ηp2 = .88. Neither model yielded a significant effect of the Affect covariate (ps ≥ .11). As evidenced by the standardized beta values, the aversion to diseased faces was magnitudinally larger in the crowding condition.

We then conducted subsidiary analyses using one-sample t-tests to determine where participants’ aversion to diseased and healthy faces were categorical aversions. We tested crowded and control participants’ contact scores separately against the scalar midpoint of 0 (i.e., neither preference nor aversion) for healthy and diseased targets (Brown, Sacco, & Medlin, 2019c). Crowding-primed participants showed neither an aversion nor a preference in their contact ratings toward healthy targets, t(241) =  − 1.62, p = .11, d =  − .15. However, these same participants demonstrated a strong aversion to diseased targets, t(241) =  − 14.50, p < .001, d =  − 1.32. Control-primed participants conversely showed an aversion to healthy targets, t(235) =  − 4.90, p < .001, d =  − .46, while showing a stronger aversion to diseased targets, t(235) =  − 15.98, p < .001, d =  − 1.47. Taken together, these findings indicate that crowd salience fosters tolerance toward physical features diagnostic of health rather than aversion to those appearing diseased.

Exploratory Interactions

Effects were further qualified by an unexpected, albeit significant, Target Status × Region interaction, b =  − .16, SE = .06, t(234.99) =  − 2.49, p = .01, β =  − .06, 95% CIβ [− .11, − .01], ηp2 = .03. We decomposed this interaction by exploring the effect of target status separately for Northern and Southern participants. For Northern participants, the effect of Target Status was significant, wherein they demonstrated more aversion toward a diseased target (M =  − 1.62, SD = 2.56) than a healthy target (M =  − .12, SD = 2.31), b =  − .75, SE = .10, t(10.65) =  − 7.52, p < .001, β =  − .29, 95% CIβ [− .37, − .22], ηp2 = .84. For Southern participants, we also found a significant effect of Target Status, wherein participants again were less willing to have contact with diseased targets (M =  − 2.85, SD = 2.18) than healthy targets (M =  − .71, SD = 2.34) b =  − 1.07, SE = .13, t(3.85) =  − 7.76, p = .002, β =  − .43, 95% CIβ [− .54, − .32], ηp2 = .94. This latter comparison was at a larger magnitude.

The Condition × Source interaction was not conventionally significant, b =  − .24, SE = .13, t(234.08) =  − 1.82, p = .07, β =  − .09, 95% CIβ [− .19, .01], ηp2 = .01, nor was the three-way Target Status × Condition × Source interaction, b = .09, SE = .06, t(234.99) = 1.37, p = .17, β = .03, 95% CIβ [− .01, .08], ηp2 = .008. This prompted no further consideration of those effects.

Discussion

We found continued evidence for how crowding shapes interpersonal preferences by considering putative cues to infectious disease. Crowd salience heightened preferences for conspecifics who appeared healthy over those who appeared diseased. However, this preference appears driven by reduced aversion to healthy conspecifics within a crowded ecology rather than enhanced aversion to diseased faces. Participants downregulated their aversion to healthy faces when crowding was salient, which could be in the service of ensuring closer proximity to those appearing most capable of satisfying disease avoidance goals (i.e., healthy; Brown, 2021; Brown & Sacco, 2019; Young et al., 2011). Conversely, the signal value of disease cues could have been pervasive across conditions, resulting in both crowding-primed and control-primed participants similarly viewing the diseased face as aversive.

Given the overall aversion to both categories of faces within a crowded environment, these findings may explain why healthy faces were not rated as categorically favorable. It could be argued this preference exists as more of a tolerance toward these features, providing a degree of nuance for our preference prediction. The inevitability of interpersonal contact in crowds may motivate individuals to identify relatively safer conspecifics within these ecologies (see Maeng et al., 2013; Sng et al., 2017), which would ensure optimal fitness enhancement around those who would be less likely to induce high infection costs compared to those who appear diseased.

These findings may clarify inconsistencies in previous work by considering whether the features putatively connote infection risk. Although BIS responses typically operate through overgeneralizations of poor health as virulent (Haselton & Nettle, 2006), crowding-induced responses may be specific to infection cues given the difficulty one may have with avoiding all disease vectors in a crowded environment. The preference for facial symmetry is rooted in perceptions of enhanced health, given its association with immunological functioning (e.g., Thornhill & Gangestad, 2006; Young et al., 2011; cf. Pound et al., 2014). Conversely, previous work demonstrates crowd salience did not heighten aversion toward obesity (Brown & Sacco, 2019), another physical cue inferred as diseased despite posing no infection risk (Miller & Maner, 2012). The blemishes exhibited by targets in this study could have been inferred as diagnostic of infection risk, necessitating crowding-primed participants to upregulate tolerance of interpersonal contact with conspecifics who could pose less disease risk.

Exploratory Regional Effects

An exploratory component of our study indicated unexpected regional differences toward pathogenic cues. Southern participants reported greater aversion to faces possessing disease cues compared to Northern participants. This difference could reflect the former’s awareness of higher environmental pathogen loads in the Southeastern U.S. compared to other regions (Feigenbaum, Muller, & Wrigley-Field, 2019; Reif et al., 2017), which could manifest as previously reported reduced tolerance for non-normative conspecifics across different cultures (Fincher & Thornhill, 2012; Murray et al., 2011). Although aversion to disease was apparent for both regions, Southern participants’ interpersonal wariness was larger, potentially reflecting an overgeneralization to potential infection risks (Haselton & Nettle, 2006).

Crowd salience affected both regions’ aversion similarly. This similarity could be rooted in a general ubiquity of the unique pathogenic threat imposed by crowding that fosters greater interpersonal restrictiveness regardless of one’s region (Maeng et al., 2013; Sng et al., 2017). Individuals could similarly valuate crowds as threatening across cultures given the role of interpersonal contact in disease transmission (Jones et al., 2008).

Alternatively, despite being similar to findings from before 2020 (Brown & Sacco, 2019), the fact that this study was conducted during the early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic could have heightened the salience of the interpersonal costs of extensive interpersonal contact unilaterally. It could have further been possible that the relatively heightened aversion to diseased faces at the Southern university could be partially explained by the fact that Northern states were already employing more extensive social distancing measures at this point in the pandemic than were Southern states (i.e., April and March 2020) to where immediate concerns of infection through disease cues could have elicited less aversion from participants in the Northern state. Nonetheless, it remains crucial to remember that the psychological responses to a pandemic may not necessarily be the same as the responses toward proximal disease cues to which humans evolved, given the historical recency of pandemics for the species (Ackerman, Tybur, & Blackwell, 2021). Potential ambiguities from the pandemic necessitate future research to clarify regional effect, which could inform policies to leverage the interplay between affiliative and disease avoidance motives in shaping adherence to public health guidelines (Young, Brown, & Sacco, 2021).

Limitations and Future Directions

Several limitations arise in this study that warrant future research. First, it is unclear how these findings translate into actual crowding experiences. Despite the current paradigm previously demonstrating considerable efficacy in activating the salience of existing within a crowd (e.g., Brown & Sacco, 2019; Maeng et al., 2013), it may not address actual behaviors individuals may employ within crowds. Future research could consider placing participants in densely populated rooms before evaluating others or potentially have researchers identify the distance participants may stand from confederates displaying varying degrees of health (e.g., Aiello et al., 1977; Baum & Valins, 1979). Confederates could additionally start coughing or sneezing while interacting with participants (Lee et al., 2010), which could further provide a more ancestrally relevant response to acute disease threat based on the presence of physical disgust cues (Kupfer & Fessler, 2018).

It should be further noted that effects seeking to activate behavioral immune system responses through motivational priming have recently experienced mixed results, even those employing the same paradigm (e.g., Brown & Sacco, 2016, 2019; Makhanova et al., 2019). Although the previous findings could reflect unforeseen nuance in understanding how disease primes could affect specific cognitions and perceptions (e.g., moral foundation endorsement is less malleable to situations, crowd salience is most robust in social perceptions), it remains necessary to identify the robustness of significant findings in the literature, especially considering smaller effect sizes in these studies. This concern makes direct replications, including the use of registered reports, necessary for many behavioral immune system studies.

Another potential limitation may concern the nature of the physical cues to disease displayed. Although connoting illness, these features are nonetheless approximations of actual disease cues. A future study could present images of targets actually infected with diseases and determine whether crowd salience heightens existing acuity to the infectious disease individuals possess, even without conspicuous sores (Axelsson et al., 2018; Tskhay, Wilson, & Rule, 2016). Crowd salience should heighten preferences for faces not exhibiting disease cues. Though not putatively associated with infection risk, it could additionally be possible that heuristic associations between disease and foreign individuals could similarly heighten aversion to members of specific outgroups (e.g., Brown, Sacco, & Medlin, 2019d; Moran et al., 2021; Navarrete & Fessler, 2006). Future research could render crowding salient before tasking participants with evaluating targets from various ingroup or outgroup categories (i.e., foreign versus domestic individuals) while assessing the degree targets appear pathogenically threatening.

Conclusion

Population density in modern life is at odds with evolutionary history and serves to motivate various threat detection responses. One response includes identifying disease threats that pose infection risk through interpersonal contact. This work found additional evidence crowding heightens acuity toward disease cues to reduce infection risk in these environments.