Skip to main content
Log in

Common and Intraverbal Bidirectional Naming

  • Special Section: The Intraverbal Relation
  • Published:
The Analysis of Verbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Naming has been defined as a generalized operant that combines speaker and listener behaviors within the individual. The purpose of this paper is to reintroduce the concept of naming and its subtypes, common and intraverbal, distinguish it from other terms such as the tact relation, and discuss the role of naming in the development of verbal behavior. Moreover, a taxonomical change is proposed. The addition of the qualifier bidirectional would serve to emphasize the speaker-listener bidirectional relation and serve to distinguish the technical term from its commonsense use. It is hoped that this paper will inspire future basic and applied research on an important extension of Skinner’s analysis of verbal behavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Subsequent equivalence studies have used the term naming in the same fashion (e.g., da Costa, Grisante, Domeniconi, de Rose, & de Souza, 2013; Hayashi, Schmidt, & Saunders, 2013).

  2. Research suggests that children first learn context-bound words (Harris, Barrett, Jones & Brookes, 1988), with nouns (usually names of things) before verbs (Gentner, 1978; 1982).

  3. Joint attention develops between the ages of 9 and 15 months and seems crucial in the development of verbal behavior (Carpenter, Nagell, Tomasello, Butterworth & Moore, 1998); although see Lieven (1994).

  4. Note the importance of generalized imitation, and more generally, maternal responsiveness of vocalizations in the development of verbal behavior (Hart & Risley, 1995).

  5. See Tamis-LeMonda, Bornstein, and Baumwell (2001) for a study on the effects of maternal responsiveness to children’s vocalizations on language development.

  6. Doug Greer pointed out to me that the term “transformation” of stimulus control may be best here. The control is not transferred from one stimulus to the other; rather the original stimulus retains its function, while the other stimulus function is “transformed” in a sense that it acquires control over a new verbal topography (tact).

  7. “Caminhão” is the word for “Truck” in Portuguese.

  8. It could be suggested that this response is not verbal (Barnes-Holmes, Barnes-Holmes, & Cullinan, 2000).

  9. For didactic purposes, my example involves the same stimuli used in Ma et al. (2016), even though Jennings and Miguel (in press) used different stimulus sets.

  10. It is possible to argue that novel categorization is a direct product of the same history of reinforcement responsible for the development of naming as a generalized operant. Although this perspective sees naming as a form of relational responding between words and their referents (Hayes, 1996), naming may still be considered a precursor for other types of relational responding. Evidence (and common sense) suggests that verbal behavior may be used (as a problem-solving strategy) to mediate adult performances in derived relations tests (e.g., Miguel et al., 2015; Ma et al., 2016).

  11. Dave Palmer suggested that rehearsal is just one common example of a source of stimulus control, but it is not always required (see Palmer, 2006).

  12. I cannot count the times I have been asked to explain the differences between tact and naming, sometimes after one of my talks on the subject. I take partial responsibility for not being more didactic. Thus, the current (hopefully didactic) paper should be taken as an apology.

References

  • Baer, D. M., Peterson, R. F., & Sherman, J. A. (1967). The development of imitation by reinforcing behavioral similarity to a model. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 10, 405–416. doi:10.1901/jeab.1967.10-405.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1968). Some current dimensions of applied behavior analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 91–97. doi:10.1901/jaba.1968.1-91.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Cullinan, V. (2000). Relational frame theory and Skinner’s verbal behavior: a possible synthesis. The Behavior Analyst, 23, 69–84.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Benedict, H. (1979). Early lexical development: comprehension and production. Journal of Child Language, 6, 183–200. doi:10.1017/S0305000900002245.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bosch, S., & Hixson, M. D. (2004). The final piece to a complete science of behavior: behavior development and behavioral cusps. The Behavior Analyst Today, 5, 244–254. doi:10.1037/h0100033.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carnerero, J. J., & Pérez-González, L. A. (2014). Induction of naming after observing visual stimuli and their names in children with autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35, 2514–2526. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2014.06.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carnerero, J. J., & Pérez-González, L. A. (2015). Emergence of naming relations and intraverbals after auditory stimulus pairing. The Psychological Record, 65, 509–522. doi:10.1007/s40732-015-0127-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., Tomasello, M., Butterworth, G., & Moore, C. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63, 1–174. doi:10.2307/1166214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carr, J. E., & Miguel, C. F. (2013). The analysis of verbal behavior and its therapeutic applications. In G. J. Madden (Ed.), APA handbook of behavior analysis (Vol. 2, pp. 329–352). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charlop, M. H. (1983). The effects of echolalia on acquisition and generalization of receptive labeling in autistic children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 16, 111–126. doi:10.1901/jaba.1983.16-111.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Clough, C. W., Meyer, C. S., & Miguel, C. F. (2016). The effects of joint control and blocking tasks on sequencing visual stimuli. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior. doi: 10.1007/s40616-016-0067-1.

  • da Costa, A. A., Grisante, P. C., Domeniconi, C., de Rose, J. C., & de Souza, D. G. (2013). Naming new stimuli after selection by exclusion. Paidéia, 23, 217–224. doi:10.1590/1982-43272355201309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cuvo, A. J. (2003). On stimulus generalization and stimulus classes. Journal of Behavioral Education, 12, 77–83. doi:10.1023/A:1022374406394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dube, W. V., MacDonald, R. P. F., Mansfield, R. C., Holcomb, W. L., & Ahearn, W. H. (2004). Toward a behavioral analysis of joint attention. The Behavior Analyst, 27, 197–207.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Eby, C. M., Greer, R. D., Tullo, L. D., Baker, K. A., & Pauly, R. (2010). Effects of multiple exemplar instruction on the transformation of stimulus function across written and vocal spelling instruction responses by students with autism. The Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Applied Behavior Analysis, 5, 20–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiorile, C. A., & Greer, R. D. (2007). The induction of naming in children with no prior tact responses as a function of multiple exemplar histories of instruction. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 23, 71–87.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Floor, P., & Akhtar, N. (2006). Can 18-month-old infants learn words by listening in on conversations? Infancy, 9, 327–339. doi:10.1207/s15327078in0903_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilic, L., & Greer, R. D. (2011). Establishing naming in typically developing two-year-old children as a function of multiple exemplar speaker and listener experiences. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 27, 157–177.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., & Longano, J. (2010). A rose by naming: how we may learn how to do it. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 26, 73–106.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., & Ross, D. E. (2008). Verbal behavior analysis: introducing and expanding new verbal capabilities in children with language delays. Boston, MA: Pearson Education, Inc..

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., Stolfi, L., Chavez-Brown, M., & Rivera-Valdez, C. (2005). The emergence of the listener to speaker component of naming in children as a function of multiple exemplar instruction. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 21, 123–134.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., Stolfi, L., & Pistoljevic, N. (2007). Emergence of naming in preschoolers: a comparison of multiple and single exemplar instruction. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 8, 109–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., Yuan, L., & Gautreaux, G. (2005). Novel dictation and intraverbal responses as a function of a multiple exemplar history. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 21, 99–116.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Grow, L. L., & Van Der Hijde, R. (2016). A comparison of procedures for teaching receptive labeling of sight words to a child with autism spectrum disorder. Behavior Analysis in Practice. doi:10.1007/s40617-016-0133-0.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M., Barrett, M., Jones, D., & Brookes, S. (1988). Linguistic input and early word meaning. Journal of Child Language, 15, 77–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, B. M., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday life of America’s children. Baltimore: Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayashi, Y., Schmidt, A. C., & Saunders, K. J. (2013). Effects of letter identification training on letter naming in prereading children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46, 838–843. doi:10.1002/jaba.90.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C. (1996). Developing a theory of derived stimulus relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 309–311. doi:10.1901/jeab.1996.65-309.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hixson, M. D. (2004). Behavioral cusps, basic behavioral repertoires, and cumulative-hierarchical learning. The Psychological Record, 54, 387–403.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horne, P. J., & Lowe, C. F. (1996). On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 185–241. doi:10.1901/jeab.1996.65-185.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Horne, P. J., Hughes, J. C., & Lowe, C. F. (2006). Naming and categorization in young children: IV: listener behavior training and transfer of function. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 85, 247–273. doi:10.1901/jeab.2006.125-04.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Horne, P. J., Lowe, C. F., & Harris, F. D. A. (2007). Naming and categorization in young children: V. Manual sign training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 87, 367–381. doi:10.1901/jeab.2007.52-06.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Horne, P. J., Lowe, C. F., & Randle, V. R. L. (2004). Naming and categorization in young children: II. Listener behavior training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 81, 267–288. doi:10.1901/jeab.2004.81-267.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Jennings, A. M., & Miguel, C. F. (in press). Training intraverbal bidirectional naming to establish generalized equivalence class performances. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior.

  • Kaplan, E. F., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (2000). The Boston naming test (2nd ed.). Philadelphia: Lippincott, Williams, & Wilkins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kobari-Wright, V. V., & Miguel, C. F. (2014). The effects of listener training on the emergence of categorization and speaker behavior in children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 47, 431–436. doi:10.1002/jaba.115.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LaFrance, D. L., & Miguel, C. F. (2014). Teaching language to children with autism spectrum disorder. In P. Sturmey, J. Tarbox, D. R. Dixon, & J. L. Matson (Eds.), Handbook of early intervention for autism spectrum disorders: research, practice, and policy (pp. 403–436). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leaf, R., & McEachin, J. (1999). A work in progress. New York: DRL Books, Inc..

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, G. P., Miguel, C. F., Darcey, E. K., & Jennings, A. M. (2015). A further evaluation of the effects of listener training on the emergence of speaker behavior and categorization in children with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders., 19, 72–81. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2015.04.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leung, J. P., & Wu, K. I. (1997). Teaching receptive naming of Chinese characters to children with autism by incorporating echolalia. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 59–68. doi:10.1901/jaba.1997.30-59.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lieven, E. V. M. (1994). Crosslinguistic and crosscultural aspects of language addressed to children. In C. Gallaway & B. J. Richards (Eds.), Input and interaction in language acquisition (pp. 56–73). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lovaas, O. I. (2003). Teaching individuals with developmental delays: basic intervention techniques. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, C. F., Horne, P. J., Harris, F. D. A., & Randle, V. R. L. (2002). Naming and categorization in young children: vocal tact training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78, 527–549. doi:10.1901/jeab.2002.78-527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, C. F., Horne, P. J., & Hughes, J. C. (2005). Naming and categorization in young children: III. Vocal tact training and transfer of function. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 83, 47–65. doi:10.1901/jeab.2005.31-04.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lowenkron, B. (1991). Joint control and the generalization of selection-based verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 9, 121–126.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lowenkron, B. (1996a). A developmental account of joint control in the idiom of Horne & Lowe (1996). Retrieved from: http://web.calstatela.edu/faculty/zlowenk/JC_H&L_paper/JC_&_h&L.html

  • Lowenkron, B. (1996b). Joint control and word-object bidirectionality. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 252–255. doi:10.1901/jeab.1996.65-252.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lowenkron, B. (1997). The role of joint control in the development of naming. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 68, 244–247. doi:10.1901/jeab.1997.68-244.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lowenkron, B. (1998). Some logical functions of joint control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 69, 327–354. doi:10.1901/jeab.1998.69-327.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lowenkron, B. (2004). Meaning: a verbal behavior account. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 20, 77–97.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ma, M. L., Miguel, C. F., & Jennings, A. M. (2016). Training intraverbal naming to establish equivalence class performances. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 105, 409–426. doi:10.1002/jeab.203.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mahoney, A. M., Miguel, C. F., Ahearn, W. H., & Bell, J. (2011). The role of common motor responses in stimulus categorization by preschool children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 95, 237–262. doi:10.1901/jeab.2011.95-237.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Michael, J. (1996). Separate repertoires or naming? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 296–298. doi:10.1901/jeab.1996.65-296.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Michael, J., Palmer, D. C., & Sundberg, M. L. (2011). The multiple control of verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 27, 3–22.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Miguel, C. F., & Kobari-Wright, V. V. (2013). The effects of tact training on the emergence of categorization and listener behavior in children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46, 669–773. doi:10.1002/jaba.62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miguel, C. F., & Petursdottir, A. I. (2009). Naming and frames of coordination. In R. A. Rehfeldt & Y. Barnes-Holmes (Eds.), Derived relational responding: applications for learners with autism and other developmental disabilities (pp. 128–144). Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publications, Inc..

    Google Scholar 

  • Miguel, C. F., Frampton, S. E., Lantaya, C. A., LaFrance, D. L., Quah, K., Meyer, C. S., Elias, N. C., & Fernand, J. K. (2015). The effects of tact training on the development of analogical reasoning. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 104, 96–118. doi:10.1002/jeab.167.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miguel, C. F., Petursdottir, A. I., Carr, J. E., & Michael, J. (2008). The role of naming in stimulus categorization by preschool children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 89, 383–405. doi:10.1901/jeab.2008-89-383.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Olenick, D. L., & Pear, J. J. (1980). Differential reinforcement of correct responses to probes and prompts in picture-name training with severely retarded children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 13, 77–89. doi:10.1901/jaba.1980.13-77.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, D. C. (2006). Joint control: a discussion of recent research. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 22, 209–2015.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, D. C. (2014). Verbal behavior. In F. K. McSweeney & E. S. Murphy (Eds.), Handbook of operant and classical conditioning (pp. 367–391). Chichester, UK: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pérez-González, L. A., Cereijo-Blanco, N., & Carnerero, J. J. (2014). Emerging tacts and selections from previous learned skills: a comparison between two types of naming. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 30, 184–192. doi:10.1007/s40616-014-0011-1.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Petursdottir, A. I., & Carr, J. E. (2011). A review of recommendations for sequencing receptive and expressive language. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44, 859–876. doi:10.1901/jaba.2011.44-859.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Petursdottir, A. I., Carp, C. L., Peterson, S. P., & Lepper, T. L. (2015). Emergence of visual-visual conditional discriminations following intraverbal training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 103, 332–348. doi:10.1002/jeab.136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ribeiro, D. M., Miguel, C. F., & Goyos, A. C. (2015). The effects of listener training on the discriminative control by elements of compound stimuli in children with disabilities. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 104, 48–62. doi:10.1002/jeab.161.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Santos, P. M., Ma, M. L., & Miguel, C. F. (2015). Training intraverbal naming to establish matching-to-sample performances. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 31, 162–182. doi:10.1007/s40616-015-0040-4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Saunders, K. J., & Spradlin, J. E. (1996). Naming and equivalence relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 304–308. doi:10.1901/jeab.1996.65-304.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1971). Reading and auditory-visual equivalences. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 14, 5–13. doi:10.1044/jshr.1401.05.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1994). Equivalence relations and behavior: a research story. Boston: Authors Cooperative.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Spears, R. A. (2002). NTC’s pocket dictionary of words and phrases. NY: McGraw -Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sprinkle, E. C., & Miguel, C. F. (2012). The effects of listener and speaker training on emergent relations in children with autism. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 28, 111–117.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sundberg, M. L., & Partington, J. W. (1998). Teaching language to children with autism or other developmental disabilities. Pleasant Hill, CA: Behavior Analysts, Inc..

    Google Scholar 

  • Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., & Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children’s achievement of language milestones. Child Development, 72, 748–767.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78, 705–722. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01025.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vouloumanos, A., & Werker, J. F. (2007). Listening to language at birth: evidence for a bias for speech in neonates. Developmental Science, 10, 159–164. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00549.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welch, S. J., & Pear, J. J. (1980). Generalization of naming responses to objects in the natural environment as a function of training stimulus modality with retarded children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 13, 629–644. doi:10.1901/jaba.1980.13-629.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Caio F. Miguel.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

Additional information

This paper is dedicated to the 20th anniversary of Horne and Lowe’s (1996) seminal paper on naming. I would like to thank Danielle LaFrance, Dave Palmer, Doug Greer, and Svea Love for their comments.

This article is part of a special section on the intraverbal relation in The Analysis of Verbal Behavior.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Miguel, C.F. Common and Intraverbal Bidirectional Naming. Analysis Verbal Behav 32, 125–138 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40616-016-0066-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40616-016-0066-2

Keywords

Navigation