Skip to main content
Log in

Association but not Recognition: an Alternative Model for Differential Imitation from 0 to 2 Months

  • Published:
Review of Philosophy and Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Skepticism toward the existence of neonatal differential imitation is fostered by views that assign it an excessive significance, making it foundational for social cognition. Moreover, a misleading theoretical framework may generate unwarranted expectations about the kinds of findings experimentalists are supposed to look for. Hence we propose a theoretical analysis that may help experimentalists address the empirical question of whether early differential imitation really exists. We distinguish three models of early imitation. The first posits automatic visuo-motor links evolved for sociocognitive functions and we call it Genetically Programmed Direct Matching (GPDM). The second is Meltzoff and Moore’s Active Intermodal Matching (AIM), which postulates a comparison between the acts of self and other. The third is the alternative we propose and we call it “Association by Similarity Theory” (AST), as it relies on the tacit functioning of this domain-general process. AST describes early imitation merely as the differential induction or elicitation of behaviors that already tend to occur spontaneously. We focus on the contrast between AIM and AST, and argue that AST is preferable to AIM for two reasons. First, AST is more parsimonious and more plausible, especially because it does not require infants to be able to recognize self-other similarities. Second, whereas the extant findings tend to disqualify AIM, AST can account for them adequately. Furthermore, we suggest that AST has the potential to give new impulse to empirical research because it discriminates promising lines of inquiry from unproductive ones.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Our definition is deliberately inclusive. Primarily, we want the definition to capture the representational basis of an impulse to a specific action. Therefore, we say that an action representation is activated even if not all the motor codes of the action are activated. Indeed, an awakened impulse may not be complied with because of antagonistic conditions and, in this case, it is reasonable to assume that not all the motor codes of the action have been activated (in this regard, see also the use of the expression “action planning” in Zoia et al. 2007). However, we also want the expression to be able to designate the processing relative to a full-fledged action execution. Here the impulse to act is complied with, all motor codes originating the action are (presumably) activated, and the proprioceptive feedback (normally) conforms to what could be expected from the action.

  2. Even Meltzoff and Moore (1994), who investigated the “unusual” tongue-protrusion-to-the-side behavior, found that, in a 90-s test period, such behavior was spontaneously produced by 6 infants out of 30 who had never seen the tongue-protrusion-to-the-side model.

  3. All three models can share the assumption that infants prefer social stimuli such as faces, eyes, or traits of the human voice to other non-social stimuli. This common assumption allows all of them to explain why it is not easy to solicit imitation with non-social stimuli (Legerstee 1991)—e.g. by stating that the non-social stimuli do not present interesting features that attract the infant’s attention. Indeed, the non-social stimuli presented by Legerstee (1991) do not present the kinds of perceptual features that are attractive to newborns (Simion et al. 2011).

  4. The distinction between AIM and a mirror neuron based model is reiterated in Meltzoff (2009, p. 38) where it is argued that the latter would not account for presumed features of NI such as “response correction” and “the imitation of novel acts.”

  5. Notoriously, terms like “nativism” or “innate” have many different meanings (Griffiths 2002; Maclaurin 2002). For example, by “innate” one might mean “existing from birth” or “something existing prior to birth” (Gallagher 2005, p. 73). This usage of the term does not distinguish between models of NI because all models must posit that the capacity to activate corresponding action representations exists at birth. We submit that notion of nativism we adopt in this paper is a pregnant one because it helps distinguish different models of NI.

  6. Cf. Meltzoff (2002, p. 24; 2005, p. 72) and Meltzoff and Moore (1997, p. 180 and 182; 1999, p. 52).

  7. See Kugiumutzakis (1999) for an exception.

  8. “Because any object or situation experienced by an individual is unlikely to recur in exactly the same form and context, psychology’s first general law should, I suggest, be a law of generalization” (Shepard 1987, p. 1317).

  9. The functioning of association by similarity can be highlighted in a classic example of Piagetian assimilation, the child who, looking at a zebra, says “that’s a horse.”

  10. Perhaps James provided us with the model of any neural formulation of association by similarity when he hypothesized that the evocation of an object occurs when a stimulus is “due to a brain-process some of whose elements awaken through habit some of the elements of the brain-process of the object which comes to view” (1981, p. 556).

  11. Obviously, it is possible to compare the present object with another object. For example, after I see a car parked on the street, when I walk pass by it I can think, “This car is like my friend’s car because it has just two doors.” However, this kind of comparison is not necessary to see the car as a car in the first place.

  12. Keven and Akins 2016, sect. 7, para. 2

  13. The tactile mode can also contribute to the infant’s registration of the relative positions of organs in the body and of the usual configurations they assume. For example, the fetus can touch its face, mouth, nose and eyes and experience the disposition of these organs (Kurjak et al. 2004); such a “tactile image” may be integrated in the experience of spatial positions acquired through proprioception.

  14. In footnote 2, we noted that Meltzoff and Moore (1994) found that infants produce TP-to-the-side spontaneously. Keven and Akins (2016) suggest that TP-to-the-side belongs to the action repertoire of the fetus eight weeks before birth. Nonetheless, we should not exclude a priori that an infant might imitate TP-to-the-side before having executed it spontaneously. AST would explain this circumstance as follows. It may be that the perceived action presents features that are experienced to different degrees in different actions already accomplished by the subject, but none of these actions instantiated all of them. This may lead to the activation of a bodily part that is more strongly associated with some features presented by the model than the bodily part involved in the corresponding action (the laterality of TP-to-the-side activates head lateral movement—cf. Meltzoff and Moore 1997, p. 182). However, with time, the fact that action features are awakened that do not correspond to any particular spontaneous action may lead to a new configuration in action planning (a new Gestalt: TP-to-the-side), so that a new action is extrapolated and induced starting from an action repertoire.

  15. We thank an anonymous reviewer for raising this point.

  16. As a consequence of being more parsimonious from a cognitive-psychological point of view, AST is also more biologically plausible. Both AST and AIM require that brain processes encoding a specific visual model activate brain processes encoding the corresponding action. Otherwise imitation could not occur. In addition to this, AIM postulates an “equivalence detector” that produces “match” or “mismatch” outputs. Where is the equivalence detector, where are the comparison outputs located in the brain? These postulates look particularly cumbersome from a biological point of view. Moreover, AIM suggests that the information characterizing each action is encoded twice, i.e. as input from vision and as input from proprioception. In contrast, AST merely entails that visual processes are associated with corresponding motor process so that the former can activate the latter. Hence the action-characterizing information can be encoded only once, in the areas where the two kinds of processes overlap. For these reasons, AST can be more easily substantiated by neuroscientific research than AIM.

  17. The idea that NI does not fulfill any decisive socio-cognitive or social function is supported by the fact that, contrary to expectations, Coulon et al. (2013) found that newborns did not imitate more when the modeled gesture was accompanied by the “congruent” auditory social stimulus (e.g. mouth opening with sound [a]). However, as anticipated in footnote 3, it is clear that AST does not exclude the existence of innate social propensities of other kinds. For example, it is compatible with Coulon et al.’s (2013) finding that newborns looked significantly longer at “audiovisual congruent” stimuli and with the hypothesis that newborns may have an innate preference for these kinds of stimuli.

  18. It is not so much that newborns are split into “imitators” or “non-imitators” (Simpson et al. 2014), although it is true that some infants may have greater motor and visual capacities and may have a greater inclination for social stimuli than others. Rather, there are many subtle factors that affect the behavior of infants and these factors may impede the imitative response. Along these lines, AST can elegantly accommodate Jones’ (2009) observations concerning tongue protrusion as arousal response. In AST arousal can still function as a reinforcing motivation for tongue-protrusion execution, which would help explain the predominance of tongue-protrusion in NI studies.

  19. We take it that it is difficult to elicit in an infant from 0 to 8 weeks an action that it is very little disposed to execute. In this case, awakening the representations of certain morphokinetic events would constitute too weak of a motivation, almost negligible, compared to other tendencies that may condition the infant at the given moment. The activation of those representations is much more likely to be effective in bringing about the action if it is combined with a certain easiness of activation of the motor components of the action, i.e. if it is combined with a spontaneous action inclination. Note that by itself a spontaneous action inclination may often not be enough to provoke the relevant action because an infant may be conditioned by antagonistic inclinations, or by a variety of internal and external stimuli. In NI the activation of components of an action representation through vision will combine with the easiness of activation of the motor components of that action representation so as to modulate action execution. Specifically, the representation of specific morphokinetic features will more effectively activate the motor components with which it has been associated in spontaneous behavior because those motor components are already characterized by a certain easiness of activation.

  20. The number for tongue protrusion was retrieved and averaged across the following studies: Heimann and Schaller 1985; Heimann et al. 1989; Meltzoff and Moore 1989; Nagy et al. 2013; Ullstadius 1998; that for mouth opening across the following: Coulon et al. 2013; Heimann and Schaller 1985; Heimann et al. 1989. Compare these frequencies with that of, for example, spontaneous lip movements, i.e. 0.14 per minute (Ekman and Rosenberg 1997). Frequencies for the other gestures can be found in other studies and are significantly lower than those for tongue protrusion and mouth opening (Meltzoff and Moore 1989; Kurjak et al. 2004; Oostenbroek et al. 2016).

  21. More precisely, “internal validity” refers to the degree of certainty that manipulation of the independent variable is responsible for observed changes in the dependent variable.

  22. One could argue that there can be better implementations of AIM than Oostenbroek et al.’ study. However, while this study can be considered as a legitimate implementation of AIM, it cannot be taken to be a legitimate implementation of AST.

References

  • Allen, C. 2012. Private codes and public structures. In The complex mind: An interdisciplinary approach, ed. D. McFarland, K. Stenning, and M. McGonigle-Chalmers, 223–242. Houndmills: Palgrave-Macmillan.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Andonotopo, W., and A. Kurjak. 2006. The assessment of fetal behavior of growth restricted fetuses by 4D sonography. Journal of Perinatal Medicine 34: 471–478.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anisfeld, M. 1991. Neonatal imitation: Review. Developmental Review 11: 60–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anisfeld, M. 2005. No compelling evidence to dispute Piaget’s timetable of the development of representational imitation in infancy. Perspectives on Imitation: From Neuroscience to Social Science 2: 107–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anisfeld, M., G. Turkewitz, S.A. Rose, F.R. Rosenberg, F.J. Sheiber, D.A. Couturier-Fagan, and I. Sommer. 2001. No compelling evidence that newborns imitate oral gestures. Infancy 2 (1): 111–122. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327078IN0201_7.

  • Barsalou, L.W. 2008. Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology 59: 617–645.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D.T., and J.C. Stanley. 1966. Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for research. Chicago: Rand-McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. 2013. Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (03): 181–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cook, R., G. Bird, C. Catmur, C. Press, and C. Heyes. 2014. Mirror neurons: From origin to function. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 37 (2): 177–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coulon, M., C. Hemimou, and A. Streri. 2013. Effects of seeing and hearing vowels on neonatal facial imitation. Infancy 18 (5): 782–796.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Elia, A., M. Pighetti, G. Moccia, and N. Santangelo. 2001. Spontaneous motor activity in normal fetuses. Early Human Development 65: 139–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Decock, L., and I. Douven. 2011. Similarity after Goodman. Review of Philosophy and Psychology 2: 61–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, H., and S. Dreyfus. 1999. The challenge of Merleau-Ponty’s phenomenology of embodiment for cognitive science. In Perspectives on embodiment: The intersection of nature and culture, ed. G. Weiss and H.F. Haber, 103–120. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., and E.L. Rosenberg. 1997. What the face reveals: Basic and applied studies of spontaneous expression using the facial action coding system (FACS). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Froese, T., and D.A. Leavens. 2014. The direct perception hypothesis: Perceiving the intention of another’s action hinders its precise imitation. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 65. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00065.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. 2005. How the body shapes the mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Giese, M.A., and G. Rizzolatti. 2015. Neural and computational mechanisms of action processing: Interaction between visual and motor representations. Neuron 88 (1): 167–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, P. 2002. What is innateness. The Monist 85 (1): 70–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hata, T., K. Kanenishi, M. Akiyama, H. Tanaka, and K. Kimura. 2005. Real-time 3-D sonographic observation of fetal facial expression. Journal of Obstetrics and Gynaecology Research 3: 337–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heimann, M. 2000. Imitation – a “fuzzy” phenomenon? In Emerging cognitive abilities in early infancy, ed. F. Lacerda, C.V. Hofsten, and M. Heimann, 231–246. Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heimann, M. 2002. Notes on individual differences and the assumed elusiveness of neonatal imitation. In The imitative mind: Development, evolution, and brain bases, 74–84.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Heimann, M., and J. Schaller. 1985. Imitative reactions among 14-21 days old infants. Infant Mental Health Journal 6 (1): 31–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heimann, M., K.E. Nelson, and J. Schaller. 1989. Neonatal imitation of tongue protrusion and mouth opening: Methodological aspects and evidence of early individual differences. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 30 (2): 90–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hepper, P.G. 2015. Behavior during the prenatal period: Adaptive for development and survival. Child Development Perspectives 9 (1): 38–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., and B. Elsner. 2009. Acquisition, representation, and control of action. In Oxford handbook of human action, eds. E. Morsella, J. A. Bargh, and P. M. Gollwitzer, 371–398. New York: Oxford University Press.

  • Hume, D. 2000. A treatise of human nature. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1999). Cartesian meditations: An introduction to phenomenology (D. Cairns, Trans.). The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff. (Original work published 1931).

  • James, W. 1981. Principles of psychology. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, S. 2009. The development of imitation in infancy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences 364: 2325–2335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katz, K., R. Mashiach, and I. Meizner. 2007. Normal range of fetal finger movements. Journal of Pediatric Orthopaedics. Part B 16: 252–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, C. 2005. Single-case designs for educational research. Boston: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keven, N., and K.A. Akins. 2016. Neonatal imitation in context: sensorymotor development in the perinatal period. Behavioral and Brain Sciences Sci 1–107. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X16000911.

  • Kratochwill, T.R. 1992. Single-case research design and analysis: An overview. In Single-case research design and analysis: New directions for psychology and education, ed. T.R. Kratochwill and J.R. Levin, 1–14. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kugiumutzakis, G. 1999. Genesis and development of early infant mimesis to facial and vocal models. In Imitation in infancy, ed. J. Nadel and G. Butterworth, 36–59. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kurjak, A., A. Tikvica, M. Stanojevic, B. Miskovic, B. Ahmed, G. Azumendi, and G.C. Di Renzo. 2008. The assessment of fetal neurobehavior by three-dimensional and four-dimensional ultrasound. The Journal of Maternal-Fetal and Neonatal Medicine 21: 675–684.

  • Kurjak, A., G. Azumendi, N. Vecek, S. Kupesic, M. Solak, D. Varga, and F. Chervenak. 2003. Fetal hand movements and facial expression in normal pregnancy studied by four-dimensional sonography. Journal of Perinatal Medicine 3: 496–508.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kurjak, A., M. Stanojevic, W. Andonotopo, A. Salihagic-Kadic, J.M. Carrera, and G. Azumendi. 2004. Behavioral pattern continuity from prenatal to postnatal life a study by four-dimensional (4D) ultrasonography. Journal of Perinatal Medicine 32: 346–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larkey, L.B., and A.B. Markman. 2005. Processes of similarity judgment. Cognitive Science 29: 1061–1076.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Legerstee, M. 1991. The role of person and object in eliciting early imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 51: 423–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lodder, P., M. Rotteveel, and M. van Elk. 2014. Enactivism and neonatal imitation: Conceptual and empirical considerations and clarifications. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 967.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maclaurin, J. 2002. The resurrection of innateness. The Monist 85: 105–130.

  • Massen, C., and W. Prinz. 2009. Movements, actions and tool-use actions: An ideomotor approach to imitation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 364: 2349–2358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A.N. 1999. Origins of theory of mind, cognition and communication. Journal of Communication Disorders 32: 251–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A.N. 2002. Imitation as a mechanism of social cognition: Origins of empathy, theory of mind, and the representation of action. In Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development, ed. U. Goswami, 6–25. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N. (2005). Imitation and other minds: The "like me" hypothesis. From: Hurley, S. & Chater, N. (Eds.), Perspectives on imitation: From neuroscience to social science. Vol. 2, pp. 55–77. Cambridge: MIT Press.

  • Meltzoff, A.N. 2007a. ‘Like me’: A foundation for social cognition. Developmental Science 10 (1): 126–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A.N. 2007b. The ‘like me’ framework for recognizing and becoming an intentional agent. Acta Psychologica 124: 26–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A.N. 2009. Roots of social cognition: The like-me framework. In Minnesota symposia on child psychology: Meeting the challenge of translational research in child psychology, vol. 35, 29–58. Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N. 2010. Bridging between action representation and infant theory of mind. In Cognition and Neuropsychology: International Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15–33. Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203845820.

  • Meltzoff, A.N. 2013. Origins of social cognition: Bidirectional self-other mapping and the “like-me” hypothesis. In Navigating the social world: What infants, children, and other species can teach us, ed. M. Banaji and S. Gelman, 139–144. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., and M.K Moore. 1983. Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Development 54: 702–709.

  • Meltzoff, A.N., and J. Decety. 2003. What imitation tells us about social cognition: A rapprochement between developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences 358 (1431): 491–500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A.N., and M.K. Moore. 1977. Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198 (4312): 75–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A.N., and M.K. Moore. 1989. Imitation in newborn infants: Exploring the range of gestures imitated and the underlying mechanisms. Developmental Psychology 25 (6): 954.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A.N., and M.K. Moore. 1992. Early imitation within a functional framework: The importance of person identity, movement, and development. Infant Behavior & Development 15: 479–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A.N., and M.K. Moore. 1994. Imitation, memory, and the representation of persons. Infant Behavior & Development 17: 83–99.

  • Meltzoff, A.N., and M.K. Moore. 1997. Explaining facial imitation: A theoretical model. Early Development & Parenting 6: 179–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. 1964a. The primacy of perception: And other essays on phenomenological psychology, the philosophy of art, history, and politics. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. 1964b. The primacy of perception. Trans. W. Cobb. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (2012). Phenomenology of perception. (D. A. Landes, Trans.). London: Routledge.

  • Meyer, K., and A. Damasio. 2009. Convergence and divergence in a neural architecture for recognition and memory. Trends in Neurosciences 32: 376–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mongillo, G. 2012. Hebbian learning. In Encyclopedia of the Sciences of Learning, ed. N. Seel. Springer Science+Business Media, LLC. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-1428-6.

  • Nagy, E., and P. Molnar. 2004. Homo imitans or homo provocans? Human imprinting model of neonatal imitation. Infant Behavior & Development 27 (1): 54–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, E., H. Kompagne, H. Orvos, and A. Pal. 2007. Gender-related differences in neonatal imitation. Infant and Child Development 16: 267–276. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.497.

  • Nagy, E., K. Pilling, H. Orvos, and P. Molnar. 2013. Imitation of tongue protrusion in human neonates: Specificity of the response in a large sample. Developmental Psychology 49 (9): 1628.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nosofsky, R.M. 1992. Similarity scaling and cognitive process models. Annual Review of Psychology 43 (1): 25–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oostenbroek, J., V. Slaughter, M. Nielsen, and T. Suddendorf. 2013. Why the confusion around neonatal imitation? A review Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology 31 (4): 328–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oostenbroek, J., T. Suddendorf, M. Nielsen, J. Redshaw, S. Kennedy-Costantini, J. Davis, et al. 2016. Comprehensive longitudinal study challenges the existence of neonatal imitation in humans. Current Biology 26 (10): 1334–1338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood (C. Attegno & F.M. Hodgson, Trans.). New York: Norton. (Original work published 1951.)

  • Piontelli, A. 2015. Development of normal fetal movements: The last 15 weeks of gestation. Milan: Springer-Verlag.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. 1990. A common coding approach to perception and action. In Relationships between perception and action: Current approaches, ed. O. Neumann and W. Prinz, 167–201. Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. 1997. Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 9: 129–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. 2002. Experimental approaches to imitation. In The imitative mind: Development, evolution, and brain basis, ed. A.N. Meltzoff and W. Prinz, 143–162. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. 2005. An ideomotor approach to imitation. In Perspectives on imitation, ed. S. Hurley and N. Chater, vol. 1, 141–156. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. 2012. Open minds: The social making of agency and intentionality. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W., G. Aschersleben, and I. Koch. 2009. Cognition and action. In Oxford handbook of human action, ed. E. Morsella, J.A. Bargh, and P.M. Gollwitzer, vol. 2, 35–71. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ray, E., and C. Heyes. 2011. Imitation in infancy: The wealth of the stimulus. Developmental Science 14 (1): 92–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reissland, N., B. Francis, J.M. Mason, and K. Lincoln. 2011. Do facial expressions develop before birth? PLoS One 6: 24081.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reissland, N., C. Mason, B. Schaal and K. Lincoln. 2012. Prenatal mouth movements: Can we identify co-ordinated fetal mouth and LIP actions necessary for feeding? International Journal of Pediatrics, 848596, 5 pages.

  • Rizzolatti, G., and C. Sinigaglia. 2010. The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: Interpretations and misinterpretations. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11 (4): 264–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, P., and T. Striano. 1999. Social cognitive development in the first year. In Early social cognition: Understanding others in the first months of life, ed. P. Rochat, 257–280. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roodenburg, P.J., J.W. Wladimiroff, and H.F. Prechtl. 1991. Classification and quantitative aspects of fetal movements during the second half of normal pregnancy. Early Human Development 25 (1): 19–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheets-Johnstone, M. 2011. The primacy of movement. 2nd ed. New York: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R.N. 1987. Toward a universal law of generalization for psychological science. Science 237: 1317–1323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simion, F., E. Di Giorgio, I. Leo, and L. Bardi. 2011. The processing of social stimuli in early infancy: From faces to biological motion perception. Progress in Brain Research 189: 173–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simpson, E.A., L. Murray, A. Paukner, and P.F. Ferrari. 2014. The mirror neuron system as revealed through neonatal imitation: Presence from birth, predictive power and evidence of plasticity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 369 (1644): 20130289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sommerville, J.A., A.L. Woodward, and A. Needham. 2005. Action experience alters 3-month-old perception of other’s actions. Cognition 96 (1): 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strid, K., T. Tjus, L. Smith, A.N. Meltzoff, and M. Heimann. 2006. Infant recall memory and communication predicts later cognitive development. Infant Behavior & Development 29 (4): 545–553. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.07.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Subiaul, F. 2010. Dissecting the imitation faculty: The multiple imitation mechanisms hypothesis. Behavioral Processes 83 (2): 222–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. 1999. The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tramacere, A., P.F. Ferrari, and A. Iriki. 2015. Epigenetic regulation of mirror neuron development, and related evolutionary hypotheses. In New frontiers in mirror neurons research, ed. P.F. Ferrari and G. Rizzolatti, 222–244. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., and K.J. Aitken. 2001. Infant intersubjectivity: Research, theory, and clinical applications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 42 (1): 3–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A. 1988. Features of similarity. In Readings in cognitive science, ed. A. Collins and E. Smith, 290–302. San Mateo: Morgan Kaufmann.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ullstadius, E. 1998. Neonatal imitation in a mother–infant setting. Early Development and Parenting 7 (1): 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Heteren, C.F., P.F. Boekkooi, H.W. Jongsma, and J.G. Nijhuis. 2000. Fetal learning and memory. Lancet 356: 1169–1170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vetter, P., and A. Newen. 2014. Varities of cognitive penetration in visual perception. Consciousness and Cognition 27: 62–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vigo, R. 2009. Modal Similarity. Journal of Experimental & Theoretical Artificial Intelligence 21 (3): 181–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vigo, R., and C. Allen. 2009. How to reason without words: Inference as categorization. Cognitive Processing 10 (1): 77–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vincini, S., Y. Jhang, E.H. Buder, and S. Gallagher. 2017. Neonatal imitation: Theory, experimental design, and significance for the field of social cognition. Frontiers in Psychology 8: 1323. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yigiter, A.B., and Z.N. Kavak. 2006. Normal standards of fetal behavior assessed by four-dimensional sonography. Journal of Maternal-Fetal and Neonatal Medicine 19: 707–721.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zoia, S., L. Blason, G. D’ottavio, M. Bulgheroni, E. Pezzetta, A. Scabar, and U. Castiello. 2007. Evidence of early development of action planning in the human fetus: A kinematic study. Experimental Brain Research 176: 217–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

SV received support from the UNAM Post-doctoral Fellowships Program.

The authors thank Shaun Gallagher, Eugene Buder, Deborah Tollefsen, Albert Newen, Yeh Hsueh, Ulrich Mueller, and Joseph Arizpe for providing detailed comments on earlier versions of this paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

SV is lead author and wrote the final version. YJ made contributions to a previous version that were integrated in the final version. YJ reviewed and accepted the final version.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stefano Vincini.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Vincini, S., Jhang, Y. Association but not Recognition: an Alternative Model for Differential Imitation from 0 to 2 Months. Rev.Phil.Psych. 9, 395–427 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-017-0373-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-017-0373-0

Navigation