Skip to main content
Log in

Implicit versus Explicit Attitudes: Differing Manifestations of the Same Representational Structures?

  • Published:
Review of Philosophy and Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Implicit and explicit attitudes manifest themselves as distinct and partly dissociable behavioral dispositions. It is natural to think that these differences reflect differing underlying representations. The present article argues that this may be a mistake. Although non-verbal and verbal measures of attitudes often dissociate (and frequently conflict), this may be because the two types of outcome-measure are differentially impacted by other factors, not because they are tapping into distinct kinds of representation or distinct storage systems. I arrive at this view through closer consideration than is usual of the mechanisms and processes that underlie overt behavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For an argument that the real-world effects of implicit attitudes are only minor ones, see Oswald et al. (2013). For a careful and measured reply, see Greenwald et al. (2015).

  2. Throughout I shall understand verbal behavior broadly, to include any form of communicative response. Hence communicating one’s attitudes towards black people on a “thermometer scale” counts as verbal, in this broad sense. The rationale is that all such behavior is subject to communicative and social norms, and is apt to engage reflective forms of cognition.

  3. In fact, I believe that all amodal beliefs (setting aside instances of seeing-as and hearing-as) are unconscious, whereas the valence and arousal components of affective states can be conscious (Carruthers 2011, 2015; see also Mandelbaum 2014). The latter point will play some role in the Section 4.

  4. Archetypal cognitive attitudes would include memories and beliefs; affective attitudes include emotions like fear and disgust, as well as feelings of desire and repulsion.

  5. Do people then change their attitudes after the questions have been asked? They will hear themselves as believing that it would be okay for tuition to be raised, for example; and so they likely accept (and come to believe) that they have such a belief. This is a new belief about their beliefs, rather than a change in their first-order attitudes. But it may still have effects on behavior just as if the latter had altered. And indeed, in one of the few investigations of the persistence of dissonance-induced attitude change, the effects on people’s expressed attitudes were still discernable a month after the initial experiment (Sénémeaud and Somat 2009).

  6. Note that unthinkingly here means without conscious consideration of alternatives, not unintentionally; on the contrary, the action results from an unconscious decision.

  7. Note that this one-representation view provides a simple and elegant explanation for the fact that implicit and explicit attitudes, while partly dissociated, are nevertheless reliably correlated. Dual-representation accounts, in contrast, are forced to appeal to other factors to explain the correlation (such as the existence of common pathways in the acquisition process, for example).

  8. Not all beliefs are explicitly stored, of course. Some are dispositions to construct such representations by inference from those that are explicitly stored, as in Dennett's (1978) example of the belief one would manifest when asked whether or not zebras in the wild wear overcoats. Never having considered the question previously, one doesn’t have a ready-stored answer. But one answers unhesitatingly nevertheless. Moreover, not all stored information should be described as a form of belief. This is because information about the statistical structure of the environment is collected and stored (perhaps associatively) for many different specialized purposes without being available to influence central decision making or verbal report. For example, it enriches the predictive models that are employed in low-level visual processing (Kok et al. 2013). There is no reason to think that stereotypes fall into this category, however. Indeed, there is good reason to think that they do not, since stereotypes are recognized by people who have them even if they aren’t consciously endorsed.

  9. This is the basis of stereotype-based humor. One could not find mother-in-law jokes funny if one was unaware of the stereotype for mothers-in-law.

  10. Although initially puzzling, in may be that this is one of the main mechanisms of evaluative learning. In highly social creatures such as ourselves it is surely adaptive to have a mechanism that can convert socially-acquired evaluative beliefs into felt values (affective dispositions). This is especially likely given that values tend to be one of the main markers that distinguish in-groups from out-groups, and given that the desires and preferences shared by most members of one’s community are likely to be adaptive ones.

  11. Hahn et al. (2014) also found that when people had their explicit attitudes re-tested following completion of the IAT, those attitudes had shifted significantly towards their implicit ones. Given the framework I have outlined this should not be surprising. For one of the motives in play in explicit-attitude reporting (albeit competing with others) is to say what one believes one’s attitude to be. Since people had recently received evidence of their racial biases through learning of their IAT results, this would exert a pressure to alter their explicit reports accordingly.

  12. Hu et al. (2017) report findings comparable to those of Gregg et al. (2006). These findings admit of a similar explanation to the one I advance here for the latter, however.

  13. In addition, it seems likely that participants would also have formed stereotype representations of the two groups through the conditioning process. For they were told at the outset that they would be learning about the characteristics of the two social groups, and they were asked to keep clear in their minds which group possessed which characteristics. A name drawn from one group paired with the word “vicious”, for example, would then lead participants to believe that members of that group are vicious.

  14. Indeed, Gilbert et al. (2012) show using fMRI and multivariate pattern analysis that the information whether black or white faces are seen can be decoded from orbitofrontal cortex when participants are making likely-friend judgments, but can be decoded from anterior medial prefrontal cortex when participants are making athleticism judgments. The former region is a classic part of the affective network, whereas the latter is often implicated in social-cognitive and stereotype judgments.

References

  • Amodio, D., and P. Devine. 2006. Stereotyping and evaluation in implicit race bias: Evidence for independent constructs and unique effects on behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 91: 652–661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aristei, S., A. Melinger, and R.A. Rahman. 2011. Electrophysiological chronometry of semantic context effects in language production. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 23: 1567–1586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Banaji, M., and A. Greenwald. 2013. Blindspot: Hidden biases of good people. New York: Delcorte Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bem, D. 1967. Self-perception: An alternative interpretation of cognitive dissonance phenomena. Psychological Review 74: 183–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Briñol, P., R. Petty, and M. McCaslin. 2009. Changing attitudes on implicit versus explicit measures: What is the difference? In Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit measures, ed. R. Petty, R. Fazio, and P. Briñol. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers, P. (2011). The Opacity of Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers, P. 2015. The centered mind: What the science of working memory shows us about the nature of human thought. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers, P. (2017). Valence and value. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research. doi: 10.1111/phpr.12395.

  • Churchland, P.M. 2012. Plato’s camera: How the physical brain captures a landscape of abstract universals. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cimpian, A., A. Brandone, and S. Gelman. 2010. Generic statements require little evidence for acceptance but have powerful implications. Cognitive Science 34: 1452–1482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, W., K. Preacher, and M. Banaji. 2001. Implicit attitude measures: Consistency, stability, and convergent validity. Psychological Science 12: 163–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. 1994. Descartes’ Error. London: Papermac.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. 1978. Brainstorms. Brighton, Sussex: Harvester Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. 1991. Consciousness explained. London: Penguin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A., and S. Chaiken. 1993. The psychology of attitudes. Boston: Wadsworth Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eichenbaum, H., M. Sauvage, N. Fortin, R. Komorowski, and P. Lipton. 2012. Towards a functional organization of episodic memory in the medial temporal lobe. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36: 1597–1608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellingsen, D.-M., J. Wessberg, M. Eikemo, J. Liljencrantz, T. Endestad, H. Olausson, and S. Leknes. 2013. Placebo improves pleasure and pain through opposite modulation of sensory processing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110: 17993–17998.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A., and P. Devine. 1994. On the motivational nature of cognitive dissonance: Dissonance as psychological discomfort. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67: 382–394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. 1957. A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. 1995. Mood and judgment. Psychological Bulletin 117: 39–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gawronski, B., and G. Bodenhausen. 2006. Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin 132: 692–731.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gawronski, B., E. Walther, and H. Blank. 2005. Cognitive consistency and the formation of interpersonal attitudes: Cognitive balance affects the encoding of social information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 41: 618–626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gendler, T. 2008. Alief in action (and reaction). Mind & Language 23: 552–585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D., and T. Wilson. 2007. Prospection: Experiencing the future. Science 317: 1351–1354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, S., J. Swencionis, and D. Amodio. 2012. Evaluative vs. trait representation in intergroup social judgments: Distinct roles of anterior temporal lobe and prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia 50: 3600–3611.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gosling, P., M. Denizeau, and D. Oberlé. 2006. Denial of responsibility: A new mode of dissonance reduction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 90: 722–733.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A., and B. Nosek. 2008. Attitudinal dissociation: What does it mean? In Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit measures, ed. R. Petty, R. Fazio, and P. Briñol. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A., M. Banaji, and B. Nosek. 2015. Statistically small effects of the implicit association test can have societally large effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 108: 553–561.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gregg, A., B. Seibt, and M. Banaji. 2006. Easier done than undone: Asymmetry in the malleability of implicit preferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 90: 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hahn, A., C. Judd, H. Hirsh, and I. Blair. 2014. Awareness of implicit attitudes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 143: 1369–1392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halberstadt, J., D. Pecher, R. Zeelenberg, L. Wai, and P. Winkielman. 2013. Two faces of attractiveness: Making beauty in averageness appear and reverse. Psychological Science 24: 2343–2346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hickok, G., and D. Poeppel. 2007. The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8: 393–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. 1997. Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist 52: 1280–1300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, W., B. Gawronski, T. Gschwendner, H. Le, and M. Schmitt. 2005. A meta-analysis on the correlation between the implicit association test and explicit self-report measures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 31: 1369–1385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, X., B. Gawronski, and R. Balas. 2017. Propositional versus dual-process accounts of evaluative conditioning: II. The effectiveness of counter-conditioning and counter-instructions in changing implicit and explicit evaluations. Social Psychology and Personality Science. doi:10.1177/1948550617691094.

  • Jussim, L. 2012. Social perception and social reality: Why accuracy dominates bias and self-fulfilling prophecy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kok, P., G. Brouwer, M. van Gerven, and F. de Lange. 2013. Prior expectations bias sensory representations in visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience 33: 16275–16284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Langland-Hassan, P. 2015. Hearing a voice as one’s own: Two views of inner-speech self-monitoring deficits in schizophrenia. Review of Philosophy and Psychology. doi:10.1007/s13164-015-0250-7.

  • LaPointe, L. 2005. Aphasia and related neurogenic language disorders. 3rd ed. New York: Theime Medical Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, J., Y. Li, P. Valdesolo, and K. Kassam. 2015. Emotion and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology 66: 799–823.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, S.J. 2014. Carving up the social world with generics. Oxford Studies in Experimental Philosophy 1: 208–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, S.J., and S. Gelman. 2012. Quantified statements are recalled as generics. Cognitive Psychology 64: 186–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, D., and P. Glimcher. 2012. The root of all value: A neural common currency for choice. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 22: 1027–1038.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, W., I. Moallem, K. Paller, and J. Gottfried. 2007. Subliminal smells can guide social preferences. Psychological Science 18: 1044–1049.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lind, A., L. Hall, B. Breidegard, C. Balkenius, and P. Johansson. 2014. Speakers’ acceptance of real-time speech exchange indicates that we use auditory feedback to specify the meaning of what we say. Psychological Science 25: 1198–1205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Madva, A., and M. Brownstein. 2017. Stereotypes, prejudice, and the taxonomy of the implicit social mind. Noûs. doi:10.1111/nous.12182.

  • Mandelbaum, E. 2014. Thinking is believing. Inquiry: An interdisciplinary journal of philosophy 57: 55–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mandelbaum, E. 2016. Attitude, inference, association: On the propositional structure of implicit bias. Noûs 50: 629–658.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matsumoto, R., D. Nair, E. LaPresto, I. Jajm, W. Bingaman, H. Shibasaki, and H. Lüders. 2004. Functional connectivity in the human language system: A cortico-cortical evoked potential study. Brain 127: 2316–2330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Novick, J., J. Trueswell, and S. Thompson-Schill. 2010. Broca’s area and language processing: Evidence for the cognitive control connection. Language and Linguistics Compass 4: 906–924.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nozari, N., G. Dell, and M. Schwartz. 2011. Is comprehension necessary for error detection? A conflict-based account of monitoring in speech production. Cognitive Psychology 63: 1–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oswald, F., G. Mitchell, H. Blanton, J. Jaccard, and P. Tetlock. 2013. Predicting ethnic and racial discrimination: A meta-analysis of IAT criterion studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 105: 171–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phelps, E., K. Lempert, and P. Sokol-Hessner. 2014. Emotion and decision making: Multiple modulatory neural circuits. Annual Review of Neuroscience 37: 263–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, M., and S. Garrod. 2013. An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36: 329–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plassmann, H., J. O’Doherty, B. Shiv, and A. Rangel. 2008. Marketing actions can modulate neural representations of experienced pleasantness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105: 1050–1054.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prasada, S. 2000. Acquiring generic knowledge. Trends in Cognitive Science 4: 66–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scher, S., and J. Cooper. 1989. Motivational basis of dissonance: The singular role of behavioral consequences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 56: 899–906.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., and G. Clore. 1983. Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 45: 513–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M., P. Railton, R. Baumeister, and C. Sripada. 2013. Navigating into the future or driven by the past. Perspectives on Psychological Science 8: 119–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sénémeaud, C., and A. Somat. 2009. Dissonance arousal and persistence in attitude change. Swiss Journal of Psychology 68: 25–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sharot, T., S. Fleming, X. Yu, R. Koster, and R. Dolan. 2012. Is choice-induced preference change long lasting? Psychological Science 23: 1123–1129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shenhav, A., M. Botvinick, and J. Cohen. 2013. The expected value of control: An integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron 79: 217–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, L., J. Greenberg, and J. Brehm. 1995. Trivialization: The forgotten mode of dissonance reduction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 68: 247–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uhlmann, E., and G. Cohen. 2005. Constructed criteria: Redefining merit to justify discrimination. Psychological Science 16: 474–480.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wager, T. 2005. The neural bases of placebo effects in pain. Current Directions in Psychological Science 14: 175–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waxman, S. 2010. Names will never hurt me? Naming and the development of racial and gender categories in preschool-aged children. European Journal of Social Psychology 40: 593–610.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I am grateful to Luca Barlassina, Greg Currie, Dan Kelly, Dan Moller, Jeremy Pober, Brent Strickland, and anonymous reviewers for comments on earlier drafts of this paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Peter Carruthers.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Carruthers, P. Implicit versus Explicit Attitudes: Differing Manifestations of the Same Representational Structures?. Rev.Phil.Psych. 9, 51–72 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-017-0354-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-017-0354-3

Navigation