Skip to main content
Log in

Does Feeling Happy Contributes to Flexible Thinking: Exploring the Association Between Positive Emotions and Cognitive Flexibility

  • Research in Progress
  • Published:
Psychological Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined the association between positive emotions and cognitive flexibility. The study comprises of two experiments which examine the influence of positive/neutral writing and watching positive movie (methods of emotion induction) on cognitive flexibility. Cognitive flexibility was observed on a shape detection task. The results do not lend support to the association between positive emotions and cognitive flexibility. The results are discussed in light of the existing literature and issues for further research are indicated.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. M., & Turken, A. U. (1999). A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychological Review, 106(3), 529–550.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baumann, N., & Kuhl, J. (2005). Positive affect and flexibility: overcoming the precedence of global over local processing of visual information. Motivation and Emotion, 29(2), 123–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biss, R. K. (2008). Happy distraction: Positive affect broadens attention to irrelevant information (Masters Thesis). Retrieved from https://tspace.library.utoronto.ca/bitstream/1807/17154/1/Biss_Renee_K_200811_MA_thesis.pdf.

  • Biss, R. K., Hasher, L., & Thomas, R. C. (2010). Positive mood is associated with the implicit use of distraction. Motivation and Emotion, 34(1), 73–77.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brunye, T. T., Mahoney, C. R., Augustyn, J. S., & Taylor, H. A. (2009). Emotional state and local versus global spatial memory. Acta Psychologica, 130(2), 138–146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bruyneel, L., Steenbergen, H., Hommel, B., Band, G. P. H., Raedt, R. D., & Koster, E. H. W. (2012). Happy but still focused: failures to find evidence for a mood-induced widening of visual attention. Psychological Research. doi:10.1007/s00426-012-0432-1. Advance online publication.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burton, C. M., & King, L. A. (2004). The health benefits of writing about intensely positive experiences. Journal of Research and Personality, 38(2), 150–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carvalho, J. O. (2008). Emotion and executive functioning: The effect of normal mood states on fluency tasks (Master’s thesis, University of Massachusetts). Retrieved from http://scholarworks.umass.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1166&context=theses.

  • Carvalho, J. O., & Ready, R. E. (2010). Emotion and executive functioning: the effect of normal mood states on fluency tasks. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32(3), 225–230.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chiew, K. S., & Braver, T. S. (2011). Positive affect versus reward: emotional and motivational influences on cognitive control. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(279), 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clore, G. L., & Huntsinger, J. R. (2007). How emotions inform judgment and regulate thought. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(9), 393–399.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clore, G. L., Wyer, R. S., Dienes, B., Gasper, K., Gohm, C., & Isbell, L. (2001). Affective feelings as feedback: Some cognitive consequences. In L. L. Martin & G. L. Clore (Eds.), Theories of mood and cognition: A user’s guidebook (pp. 27–62). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, S., Doyle, W. J., Turner, R. B., Alper, C. M., & Skoner, D. P. (2003). Emotional style and susceptibility to the common cold. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65(4), 652–657.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohn, M. A. (2008). Positive emotions: Short-term mechanisms, long- term outcomes, and mediating processes (Doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan). Retrieved from http://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/handle/2027.42/60754.

  • Compton, R. J., Wirtz, D., Pajoumand, G., Claus, E., & Heller, W. (2004). Association between positive affect and attentional shifting. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 28(6), 733–744.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five- Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources.

    Google Scholar 

  • Danner, D. D., Snowdon, D. A., & Friesen, W. V. (2001). Positive affect in early life and longevity: findings from the Nun study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(5), 804–813.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Das, E., & Fennis, B. M. (2008). In the mood to face the facts: when a positive mood promotes systematic processing of self-threatening information. Motivation and Emotion, 32(3), 221–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (1998). Anxiety and attentional focusing: trait, state and hemispheric influences. Personality and Individual Differences, 25(4), 745–761.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Derryberry, D., & Tucker, D. M. (1994). Motivating the focus of attention. In P. M. Niedenthal & S. Kitayama (Eds.), The heart’s eye: Emotional influences in perception and attention (pp. 167–196). San Diego: Academic.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., & Diener, C. (1996). Most people are happy. Psychological Science, 7(3), 181–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Estrada, C. A., Isen, A. M., & Young, M. J. (1997). Positive affect facilitates integration of information and decreases anchoring among physicians. Organizational Behaviour and Human Decision Processes, 72(1), 117–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fleisher, C. A. (2004). Happiness helps, but how? Does interhemispheric communication mediate the impact of positive affect on cognitive flexibility? (Bachelor’s thesis). Retrieved from http://triceratops.brynmawr.edu/dspace/handle/10066/753.

  • Forster, J. (2012). GLOMOsys: the how and why of global and local processing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 15–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 300–319.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology. American Psychologist, 56(3), 218–226.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L. (2003). The value of positive emotions. American Scientist, 91(4), 330–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London B, 359(1449), 1367–1377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought- action repertoires. Cognition and Emotion, 19(3), 313–332.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L., Mancuso, R. A., Branigan, C., & Tugade, M. M. (2000). The undoing effect of positive emotions. Motivation and Emotion, 24(4), 237–258.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L., Tugade, M. M., Waugh, C. E., & Larkin, G. R. (2003). What good are positive emotions in crisis? A prospective study of resilience and affect following the terrorist attacks on the United States on September 11th, 2001. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(2), 365–376.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gable, P., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The motivational dimensional model of affect: implications for breadth of attention, memory, and cognitive categorization. Cognition and Emotion, 24(2), 322–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Attending to the big picture: mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psychological Science, 13(1), 34–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. R. (2004). Integration of emotion and cognitive control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(2), 46–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guastella, A. J., & Dadds, M. R. (2008). Cognitive behavioural emotion writing tasks: a controlled trial of multiple processes. Journal of Behaviour Therapy, 39(4), 558–566.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harmon-Jones, E., & Gable, P. A. (2009). Neural activity underlying the effect of approach-motivated positive affect on narrowed attention. Psychological Science, 20(4), 406–409.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harrist, S., Carlozzi, B. L., McGovern, A. R., & Harrist, A. W. (2007). Benefits of expressive writing and expressive talking about life goals. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(4), 923–930.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirt, E. R., Devers, E. E., & McCrea, S. M. (2008). I want to be creative: exploring the role of hedonic contingency theory in the positive mood-cognitive flexibility link. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(2), 213–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunsinger, J. R. (2012). Does positive affect broaden and negative affect narrow attentional scope: a new answer to an old question. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. doi:10.1037/a0027709. Advance online publication.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunsinger, J. R., Clore, G. L., & Bar-Anan, Y. (2010). Mood and global–local focus: priming a local focus reverses the link between mood and global–local processing. Emotion, 10(5), 722–726.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunsinger, M., Isbell, L. M., & Clore, G. L. (2012). Sometimes happy people focus on the trees and sad people focus on the forest: context-dependent effects of mood in impression formation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(2), 220–232.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Isbell, L. M. (2010). What is the relationship between affect and information-processing styles? This and other global and local questions inspired by GLOMOsys. Psychological Inquiry, 21(3), 225–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M. (1993). Positive affect and decision making. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 261–277). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 1122–1131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jefferies, L. N., Smilek, D., Eich, E., & Enns, J. T. (2008). Emotional valance and arousal interact in the control of attention. Mood and Attention, 19(3), 290–295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, K. J., Waugh, C. E., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2010). Smile to see the forest: facially expressed positive emotions broaden cognition. Cognition and Emotion, 24(2), 299–321.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, E. K. (2006). Relationship between the five-factor model of personality and the scale of creative attributes and behavior: a validational study. Individual Differences Research, 4(5), 299–305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keltner, D., Locke, K. D., & Audrain, P. C. (1993). The influence of attribution on the relevance of negative feelings to personal satisfaction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19(1), 21–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Khan, B. E., & Isen, A. M. (1993). The influence of positive affect on variety seeking among safe, enjoyable products. The Journal of Consumer Research, 20(2), 257–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, S., Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (2007). Aging and benefit of distractibility. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(2), 301–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., & Kurokawa, M. (2000). Culture, emotion, and well-being: good feelings in Japan and the United States. Cognition and Emotion, 14(1), 93–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Konishi, C. Y. (2007). Positive emotions induced through nature-related stimuli cognitive, personality and physiological Correlates (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Database. (UMI No. 3289737).

  • Kopelman, S., & Rosette, A. S. (2007). Cultural variation in response to strategic display of emotions during negotiations. Group Decision and Negotiation, 17(1), 65–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liad, U. (2006). The extraverted and the neurotic glasses are of different colors. Personality and Individual Differences, 41(4), 745–754.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, M. (1990). On the induction of mood. Clinical Psychology Review, 10(6), 669–697.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miyamoto, Y., & Ma, X. (2011). Dampening or savoring positive emotions: a dialectical cultural script guides emotion regulation. Emotion, 11(6), 1346–1357.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mosher, C. E., & Burg, D. S. (2006). Health effects of expressive letter writing. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 25(10), 1122–1139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, N., Sujan, H., Hirt, E. R., & Sujan, M. (1990). The influence of mood on catagorization: a cognitive flexibility interpretation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(3), 411–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Navon, D. (1977). Forest before tress: the precedence of global features in visual processing. Cognitive Psychology, 9(3), 353–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noguchi, K., Gohm, C. L., & Dalsky, D. J. (2006). Cognitive tendencies of focusing on positive and negative information. Journal of Research in Personality, 40(6), 891–910.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nunnally, J. C., & Bernstein, I. H. (1994). Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olivers, C. N. L., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2006). The beneficial effects of additional task load, positive affect, and instruction on the attentional blink. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(2), 364–379.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pallant, J. (2007). SPSS survival manual: A step by step guide to data analysis using SPSS for Windows (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw Hill Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pessoa, L. (2009). How do emotion and motivation direct executive control? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(4), 160–166.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Philips, L. H., Bull, R., Adams, E., & Fraser, L. (2002). Positive mood and executive function: evidence from stroop and fluency tasks. Emotion, 2(1), 12–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Qu, L., & Zelazo, P. D. (2007). The facilitative effect of positive stimuli on 3-year- olds’ flexible rule use. Cognitive Development, 22(4), 456–473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raghunathan, R., & Trope, Y. (2002). Walking the tightrope between feeling good and being accurate: mood as a resource in processing persuasive messages. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(3), 510–525.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rottenberg, J., Ray, R., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Emotion elicitation using films. In J. A. Coan & J. B. Allen (Eds.), Handbook of emotion elicitation and assessment (pp. 9–28). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, G., Hirsh, J. B., & Anderson, A. K. (2007). Positive affect increases the breadth of attentional selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(1), 383–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sacharin, V. (2009). The influence of emotions on cognitive flexibility (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from http://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/handle/2027.42/64652/vsachari1.pdf?sequence=1.

  • Scollon, C. N., Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2004). Emotions across cultures and methods. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 35(3), 304–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, K. M., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2006). How to increase and sustain positive emotion: the effects of expressing gratitude and visualizing best possible selves. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 1(2), 73–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sibia, A., & Misra, G. (2011). Understanding emotion. In G. Misra (Ed.), Handbook of psychology in India (pp. 286–298). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spering, M., Wagener, D., & Funke, J. (2005). The role of emotions in complex problem- solving. Cognition and Emotion, 19(8), 1252–1261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Srinivasan, N., & Gupta, R. (2010). Emotion-attention interactions in recognition memory for distractor faces. Emotion, 10(2), 207–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Srinivasan, N., & Hanif, A. (2009). Global-happy and local-sad: perceptual processing affect emotion identification. Cognition and Emotion, 24(6), 1062–1069.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Srivastava, P., & Srinivasan, N. (2010). Time course of visual attention with emotional faces. Visual Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 72(2), 369–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tan, H. K., Jones, G. V., & Watson, D. G. (2009). Encouraging the perceptual underdog: positive affective priming of nonpreffered local–global processes. Emotion, 9(2), 238–247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, R. C., & Hasher, L. (2006). The influence of emotional valance on age differences in early processing and memory. Psychology and Aging, 21(4), 821–825.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trope, Y., & Neter, E. (1994). Reconciling competing motives in selfevaluation: the role of self-control in feedback seeking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(4), 646–657.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Stigchel, S., Imants, P., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2010). Positive affect increases cognitive control in the antisaccade task. Brain and Cognition, 75(2), 177–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vermeulen, N. (2010). Current positive and negative affective states modulate attention: an attentional blink study. Personality and Individual Differences, 49(5), 542–545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wadlinger, H. A., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2006). Positive mood broadens visual attention to positive stimuli. Motivation and Emotion, 30(1), 89–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, S. M., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1993). Characterization of the dopaminergic innervation of the primate frontal cortex using a dopamine-specific antibody. Cerebral Cortex, 3(3), 199–222.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yeh, Y. Y. (2010). Emotional effects on visual information processing. Taiwan: National Taiwan University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, L., & Huang, J. (2001). Thinking styles and the five-factor model of personality. European Journal of Personality, 15(6), 465–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rabindra Kumar Pradhan.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Nath, P., Pradhan, R.K. Does Feeling Happy Contributes to Flexible Thinking: Exploring the Association Between Positive Emotions and Cognitive Flexibility. Psychol Stud 59, 180–190 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12646-014-0241-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12646-014-0241-2

Keywords

Navigation