Skip to main content
Log in

The odd-ending price justification effect: the influence of price-endings on hedonic and utilitarian consumption

  • Original Empirical Research
  • Published:
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper examines how odd-ending pricing influences consumption of hedonic and utilitarian products. Four studies test the hypothesis that the discount image associated with odd-ending prices reduces anticipated guilt and provides justification for hedonic consumption – an effect the authors label the odd-ending price justification effect (OPJE). Study 1 reveals people are more likely to choose hedonic over utilitarian products when they have odd-ending prices. Study 2 finds that the effect of odd-ending prices on hedonic consumption is mediated by guilt reduction. Study 3 reveals a boundary condition for the OPJE – purchase likelihood of hedonic products increases only when monetary, not nonmonetary, guilt is reduced. Study 4 suggests the OPJE operates at an unconscious level, as consumers who are made aware of the trivial difference between odd- and round-ending prices are no longer influenced by odd-ending prices. The theoretical, practical, and research implications of these findings are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We found similar results regardless of whether the items were presented before or after the DV (Before: M different = 1.85, SD = 1.28; M affect = 3.23, SD = 2.52; After: M different = 1.63, SD = 1.36; M affect = 3.20, SD = 2.95).

References

  • Ajzen, I., Brown, T. C., & Rosenthal, L. H. (1996). Information bias in contingent valuation: effects of personal relevance, quality of information, and motivational orientation. Journal of Environmental Economics and Management, 30, 43–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, R. E., & Jolson, M. A. (1980). Technical wording in advertising: implications for market segmentation. Journal of Marketing, 44(1), 57–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Basso, M. R., Schefft, B. K., Ris, M. D., & Dember, W. N. (1996). Mood and global–local visual processing. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2(3), 249–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Batra, R., & Ahtola, O. T. (1991). Measuring the hedonic and utilitarian sources of consumer attitudes. Marketing Letters, 2(2), 159–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F. (2002). Yielding to temptation: self-control failure, impulsive purchasing, and consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 28(4), 670–676.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bizer, G. Y., & Schindler, R. M. (2005). Direct evidence of ending‐digit drop‐off in price information processing. Psychology & Marketing, 22(10), 771–783.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chakraborty, G., Allred, A., Sukhdial, A. S., & Bristol, T. (1997). Use of negative cues to reduce demand for counterfeit products. In M. Brucks & D. J. MacInnis (Eds.), Advances in consumer research (pp. 345–349). Provo: Association for Consumer Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chitturi, R., Raghunathan, R., & Mahajan, V. (2008). Delight by design: the role of hedonic versus utilitarian benefits. Journal of Marketing, 72(3), 48–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Choi, J., Lee, K., & Ji, Y. (2012). What type of framing message is more appropriate with nine-ending pricing? Marketing Letters, 23(3), 603–614.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coulter, K. S. (2001). Odd-ending price underestimation: an experimental examination of left-to-right processing effects. Journal of Product & Brand Management, 10(5), 276–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crowley, A. E., Spangenberg, E. R., & Hughes, K. R. (1992). Measuring the hedonic and utilitarian dimensions of attitudes toward product categories. Marketing Letters, 3(3), 239–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, M. G., Murphy, P. E., & Skelly, G. U. (1986). Research note: the influence of perceived risk on brand preference for supermarket products. Journal of Retailing, 62(2), 204–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Estelami, H. (1999). The computational effect of price endings in multi-dimensional price advertising. Journal of Product & Brand Management, 8(3), 244–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitzsimons, G. J., & Williams, P. (2000). Asking questions can change choice behavior: does it do so automatically or effortfully? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 6(3), 195–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fortin, D., Cleland, S., & Jenkins, A. (2008). Effects of advertised pricing on brand image for an on-line retailer. Proceedings of the 2008 Conference of the American Academy of Advertising, 263–275.

  • Gendall, P., Holdershaw, J., & Garland, R. (1997). The effect of odd pricing on demand. European Journal of Marketing, 31(11/12), 799–813.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gedenk, K., & Sattler, H. (1999). The impact of price thresholds on profit contribution—should retailers set 9-ending prices? Journal of Retailing, 75(1), 33–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guéguen, N., & Legoherel, P. (2004). Numerical encoding and odd-ending prices: the effect of a contrast in discount perception. European Journal of Marketing, 38(1/2), 194–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, C., & Bray, J. (2007). Price endings and consumer segmentation. Journal of Product & Brand Management, 16(3), 200–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52(12), 1280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirschman, E. C., & Holbrook, M. B. (1982). Hedonic consumption: emerging concepts, methods, and proposition. Journal of Marketing, 46(3), 92–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holdershaw, J., Gendall, P., & Garland, R. (1997). The widespread use of odd pricing in the retail sector. Marketing Bulletin, 8, 53–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalyanaram, G., & Little, J. D. C. (1994). An empirical analysis of latitude of price acceptance in consumer package goods. Journal of Consumer Research, 21(3), 408–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kalyanam, K., & Shively, T. S. (1998). Estimating irregular pricing effects: a stochastic spline regression approach. Journal of Marketing Research, 35(1), 16–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keppel, G. (1982). Design and analysis: A Researcher’s Handbook (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Khan, U., & Dhar, R. (2006). Licensing effect in consumer choice. Journal of Marketing Research, 43(2), 259–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Khan, U., & Dhar, R. (2010). Price framing effects on purchase of hedonic and utilitarian bundles. Journal of Marketing Research, 47(6), 1090–1099.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kivetz, R., & Simonson, I. (2002). Earning the right to indulge: effort as a determinant of customer preferences toward frequency program rewards. Journal of Marketing Research, 39(2), 155–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kivetz, R., & Zheng, Y. (2006). Determinants of justification and self-control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(4), 572–587.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleinsasser, S., & Wagner, U. (2011). Price endings and tourism consumers’ price perceptions. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 18(1), 58–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laroche, M., Kim, C., & Zhou, L. (1996). Brand familiarity and confidence as determinants of purchase intention: an empirical test in a multiple brand context. Journal of Business Research, 37, 115–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lattin, J. M., & Bucklin, R. E. (1989). Reference effects of price and promotion on brand choice behavior. Journal of Marketing Research, 26(3), 299–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee-Wingate, S. N., & Corfman, K. P. (2010). A little something for me and maybe for you, too: promotions that relieve guilt. Marketing Letters, 21(4), 385–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liang, J., & Kanetkar, V. (2006). Price endings: magic and math. Journal of Product & Brand Management, 15(6), 377–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macé, S. (2012). The impact and determinants of nine-ending pricing in grocery retailing. Journal of Retailing, 88(1), 115–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manning, K. C., & Sprott, D. E. (2009). Price endings, left-digit effects, and choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(2), 328–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mishra, A., & Mishra, H. (2011). The influence of price discount versus bonus pack on the preference for virtue and vice foods. Journal of Marketing Research, 48(1), 196–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moorman, C., Diehl, K., Brinberg, D., & Kidwell, B. (2004). Subjective knowledge, search locations, and consumer choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(3), 673–680.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: the precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9(3), 353–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Curry, S., & Strahilevitz, M. (2001). Probability and mode of acquisition effects on choices between hedonic and utilitarian options. Marketing Letters, 12(1), 37–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Okada, E. M. (2005). Justification effects on consumer choice of hedonic and utilitarian goods. Journal of Marketing Research, 42(1), 45–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pham, M. T. (1998). Representativeness, relevance, and the use of feelings in decision making. Journal of Consumer Research, 25, 144–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poltrock, S. E., & Schwartz, D. R. (1984). Comparative judgments of multidigit numbers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10(1), 32–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., & Hayes, A. F. (2007). Addressing moderated mediation hypothesis: theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 42(1), 185–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prelec, D., & Loewenstein, G. (1998). The red and the black: mental accounting of savings and debt. Marketing Science, 17(1), 4–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quigley, C. J., Jr., & Notarantonio, E. M. (1992). An exploratory investigation of perceptions of odd and even pricing. Developments in Marketing Science, 15, 306–309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schindler, R. M. (1991). Symbolic meanings of a price ending. In R. H. Holman & M. R. Solomon (Eds.), Advances in consumer research (Vol. 18, pp. 794–801). Provo: Association for Consumer Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schindler, R. M. (2006). The 99 price ending as a signal of a low-price appeal. Journal of Retailing, 82(1), 71–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schindler, R. M., & Kibarian, T. M. (1996). Increased consumer sales response though use of 99-ending prices. Journal of Retailing, 72(2), 187–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schindler, R. M., & Kibarian, T. M. (2001). Image communicated by the use of 99 endings in advertised prices. Journal of Advertising, 30(4), 95–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schindler, R. M., & Kirby, P. N. (1997). Patterns of rightmost digits used in advertised prices: implications for nine-ending effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 24(2), 192–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schindler, R. M., & Wiman, A. R. (1989). Effects of odd pricing on price recall. Journal of Business Research, 19(3), 165–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soman, D., & Cheema, A. (2011). Earmarking and partitioning: increasing saving by low-income households. Journal of Marketing Research, 48, 14–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stiving, M., & Winer, R. S. (1997). An empirical analysis of price endings with scanner data. Journal of Consumer Research, 24(1), 57–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strahilevitz, M., & Myers, J. G. (1998). Donations to charity as purchase incentives: how well they work may depend on what you are trying to sell. Journal of Consumer Research, 24(4), 434–446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, M., & Morwitz, V. (2005). Penny wise and pound foolish: the left-digit effect in price cognition. Journal of Consumer Research, 32(1), 54–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Voss, K. E., Spangenberg, E. R., & Grohmann, B. (2003). Measuring the hedonic and utilitarian dimensions of consumer attitude. Journal of Marketing Research, 40(3), 310–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wertenbroch, K., & Dhar, R. (2000). Consumer choice between hedonic and utilitarian goods. Journal of Marketing Research, 37(1), 60–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., & Brekke, N. (1994). Mental contamination and mental correction: unwanted influences on judgments and evaluations. Psychological Bulletin, 116(1), 117–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zheng, Y., & Kivetz, R. (2009). The differential promotion effectiveness on hedonic versus utilitarian products. Advances in Consumer Research, 36, 565.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jungsil Choi.

Appendices

Appendix 1

Table 1 Summary of research on odd-ending pricing

Appendix 2

Table 2 Details of the stimuli used in Study 1

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Choi, J., Li, Y.J., Rangan, P. et al. The odd-ending price justification effect: the influence of price-endings on hedonic and utilitarian consumption. J. of the Acad. Mark. Sci. 42, 545–557 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11747-014-0369-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11747-014-0369-6

Keywords

Navigation