Skip to main content
Log in

The concreteness of titles affects metacognition and study motivation

  • Published:
Instructional Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two experiments investigated the extent to which the concreteness of titles affects metacognitive text expectations, study motivation, and comprehension test performance. Sixty-three American and 61 German students were presented with three titles (either concrete or abstract), based upon which the students estimated their expected ease-of-comprehension, and the expected interestingness, of three expository texts. Students also reported how motivated they were to study the texts. The students then studied the texts and completed comprehension tests. The results revealed that students expected texts with concrete (as opposed to abstract) titles to be easier to comprehend and more interesting, and were more motivated to study those texts. Structural Equation Modelling revealed that the effects of titles on reported study motivation were mediated by expected interestingness. In addition to that, expected interestingness and reported study motivation were partially mediated by expected ease-of-comprehension. Comprehension test performance was not affected. The results provide robust evidence for positive motivational effects of concrete titles. More specifically, the results indicate that concrete titles—which are specific and easy to imagine—promote students’ motivation to study expository texts by encouraging the students to expect that they will find the texts interesting, and that they will be able to understand the texts.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderman, E. M., & Maehr, M. L. (1994). Motivation and schooling in the middle grades. Review of Educational Research, 64, 287–309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, R. C., Goetz, E. T., Pichert, J. W., & Halff, H. M. (1977). Two faces of the conceptual peg hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 3(2), 142–149. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.3.2.142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, D. J., & Brooks, P. H. (1976). Influence of contextual organizing material on childrens’ listening comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 68, 711–716.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ausubel, D. P. (1968). Educational psychology: A cognitive view. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.1988.10885871.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, A. E., Nelson, T. O., Dunlosky, J., Mazzoni, G., & Narens, L. (1999). An integrative system of metamemory components involved in retrieval. In D. Gopher & A. Koriat (Eds.), Attention and performance XVII: Cognitive regulation of performance: Interaction of theory and application (pp. 287–313). Cambridge, MA: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Begg, I., Duft, S., Lalonde, P., Melnick, R., & Sanvito, J. (1989). Memory predictions are based on ease of processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 28(5), 610–632. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(89)90016-8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Best, R. M., Rowe, M., Ozuru, Y., & McNamara, D. S. (2005). Deep-level comprehension of science texts: The role of the reader and the text. Topics in Language Disorders, 25(1), 65–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W., & Conant, L. L. (2009). Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 19(12), 2767–2796. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp055.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, R. A. (1994). Memory and metamemory considerations in the training of human beings. In J. Metcalfe & A. P. Shimamura (Eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about knowing (pp. 185–205). Cambridge, MA: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2011). Making things hard on yourself, but in a good way: Creating desirable difficulties to enhance learning. In M. A. Gernsbacher, R. W. Pew, L. M. Hough, & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), Psychology and the real world: Essays illustrating fundamental contributions to society (pp. 56–64). New York: Worth Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bollen, K. A., & Pearl, J. (2012). Handbook of causal analysis for social research. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bouffard, T., & Narciss, S. (2011). Benefits and risks of positive biases in self-evaluation of academic competence: Introduction. International Journal of Educational Research., 50, 205–208. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2011.08.001.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bransford, J. D., & Johnson, M. K. (1972). Contextual prerequisites for understanding: Some investigations of comprehension and recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11(6), 717–726. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(72)80006-9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Britton, B. K., & Gülgöz, S. (1991). Using Kintsch’s computational model to improve instructional text: Effects of repairing inference calls on recall and cognitive structures. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83(3), 329–345. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.83.3.329.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. D., & Marshall, M. A. (2001). Self-esteem and emotion: Some thoughts about feelings. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(5), 575–584. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167201275006.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butler, D. L., & Winne, P. H. (1995). Feedback and self-regulated learning: A theoretical synthesis. Review of Educational Research, 65(3), 245–282. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0742-051X(97)80002-9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Feinstein, J. A., & Jarvis, W. B. G. (1996). Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 119(2), 197–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clyne, M. (1987). Cultural differences in the organization of academic texts: English and German. Journal of Pragmatics, 11(2), 211–241. https://doi.org/10.1016/0378-2166(87)90196-2.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coiro, J. (2003). Exploring literacy on the internet: Reading comprehension on the internet: Expanding our understanding of reading comprehension to encompass new literacies. The Reading Teacher, 56(5), 458–464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corkill, A. J., Bruning, R. H., & Glover, J. A. (1988). Advance organizers: Concrete versus abstract. The Journal of Educational Research, 82(2), 76–81. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.1988.10885871.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dai, D. Y., & Wang, X. (2007). The role of need for cognition and reader beliefs in text comprehension and interest development. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 32(3), 332–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dooling, D. J, & Mullet, R. L. (1973). Locus of thematic effects in retention of prose. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 97(3), 404–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., & Lipko, A. R. (2007). Metacomprehension a brief history and how to improve its accuracy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(4), 228–232. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00509.x.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunning, D., Heath, C., & Suls, J. M. (2004). Flawed self-assessment implications for health, education, and the workplace. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 5(3), 69–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durrell, M. (2006). Germanic Languages. In K. Brown (Ed.), Encyclopedia of language and linguistics (pp. 53–55). Amsterdam: Elsevier (North Holland Publishing Co.). https://doi.org/10.1016/B0-08-044854-2/02189-1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (1992). The development of achievement-task values: A theoretical analysis. Developmental Review, 12, 265–310.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). Motivation to succeed. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Social, emotional, and personality development in Handbook of Child Psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 1017–1096). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Filippatou, D. (1995). Effects of pictures and titles on reading accuracy and reading comprehension of primary school children, including children with specific developmental dyslexia (SpDD). Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Manchester.

  • Filippatou, D., & Pumfrey, P. D. (1995). Pictures, titles, reading accuracy and reading comprehension: A research review (1973–95). Educational Research, 38(3), 259–291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fulmer, S. M., D’Mello, S. K., Strain, A., & Graesser, A. C. (2015). Interest-based text preference moderates the effect of text difficulty on engagement and learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 41, 98–110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2014.12.005.

    Google Scholar 

  • Furnham, A. (1986). Response bias, social desirability and dissimulation. Personality and Individual Differences, 7(3), 385–400. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(86)90014-0.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, M. A. (1984). Resolving 20 years of inconsistent interactions between lexical familiarity and orthography, concreteness, and polysemy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113(2), 256–281. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.113.2.256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glanzer, M., & Bowles, N. (1976). Analysis of the word-frequency effect in recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 2(1), 21–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.2.1.21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., Wilkinson, A. C., & Epstein, W. (1982). The illusion of knowing: Failure in the self-assessment of comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 10(6), 597–602. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03202442.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A. C. (2007). An introduction to strategic reading comprehension. In D. S. McNamara (Ed.), Reading comprehension strategies: Theories, interventions, and technologies (pp. 3–26). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A. C., McNamara, D. S., Louwerse, M. M., & Cai, Z. (2004). Coh-Metrix: Analysis of text on cohesion and language. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36(2), 193–202. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195564.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. A., & Azevedo, R. (2007). A theoretical review of Winne and Hadwin’s model of self-regulated learning: New perspectives and directions. Review of Educational Research, 77(3), 334–372. https://doi.org/10.3102/003465430303953.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hacker, D. J., Dunlosky, J., & Graesser, A. C. (1998). Metacognition in educational theory and practice. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harter, S. (1981). A new self-report scale of intrinsic versus extrinsic orientation in the classroom: Motivational and informational components. Developmental Psychology, 17, 300–312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harter, S. (1985). Competence as a dimension of self-evaluation: Toward a comprehensive model of self-worth. In R. L. Leahy (Ed.), The development of the self (pp. 55–121). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hidi, S., & Baird, W. (1988). Strategies for increasing text-based interest and students’ recall of expository texts. Reading Research Quarterly, 23(4), 465–483. https://doi.org/10.2307/747644.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hidi, S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2000). Motivating the academically unmotivated: A critical issue for the 21st century. Review of Educational Research, 70(2), 151–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hidi, S., Renninger, K. A., & Krapp, A. (2004). Interest, a motivational variable that combines affective and cognitive functioning. In D. Yun Dai & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), Motivation, emotion, and cognition: Integrative perspectives on intellectual functioning and development (pp. 89–115). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, P. (2016). The meaning of ‘life’ and other abstract words: Insights from neuropsychology. Journal of Neuropsychology, 10(2), 317–343. https://doi.org/10.1111/jnp.12065.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kieras, D. E. (1980). Initial mention as a signal to thematic content in technical passages. Memory & Cognition, 8(4), 345–353. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03198274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kieras, D. E. (1981). Topicalization effects in cued recall of technical prose. Memory & Cognition, 9(6), 541–549. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03202348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (2004). The construction-integration model of text comprehension and its implications for instruction. In R. B. Ruddell & N. J. Unrau (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes of reading (5th ed., pp. 1270–1328). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • König, E., & van der Auwera, J. (Eds.). (1994). The Germanic languages. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (1997). Monitoring one’s own knowledge during study: A cue-utilization approach to judgments of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126(4), 349–370. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.126.4.349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (2000). The feeling of knowing: Some metatheoretical implications for consciousness and control. Consciousness and Cognition, 9(2), 149–171. https://doi.org/10.1006/ccog.2000.0433.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kozminsky, E. (1977). Altering comprehension: The effect of biasing titles on text comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 5(4), 482–490. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197390.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krapp, A. (1999). Interest, motivation and learning: An educational-psychological perspective. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 14(1), 23–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leopold, C., & Mayer, R. E. (2015). An imagination effect in learning from scientific text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(1), 47–63. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, J. R. (1989). A transfer-appropriate processing perspective of pictures in prose. In M. Pressley & J. R. Levin (Eds.), Cognitive strategy research: Educational applications. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lin, L.-M., Moore, D. W., & Zabrucky, K. M. (2000). Metacomprehension knowledge and comprehension of expository and narrative texts among younger and older adults. Educational Gerontology, 26(8), 737–749.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lin, L. M., & Zabrucky, K. M. (1998). Calibration of comprehension: Research and implications for education and instruction. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 23(4), 345–391. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1998.0972.

    Google Scholar 

  • Long, S. A., Winograd, P. N., & Bridge, C. A. (1989). The effects of reader and text characteristics on imagery reported during and after reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 24(3), 353–372. https://doi.org/10.2307/747774.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorch, R. F., Jr. (1989). Text-signaling devices and their effects on reading and memory processes. Educational Psychology Review, 1(3), 209–234. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01320135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorch, R. F., Jr., & Lorch, E. P. (1996). Effects of headings on text recall and summarization. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 21(3), 261–278. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1996.0022.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mabe, P. A., & West, S. G. (1982). Validity of self-evaluation of ability: A review and meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67(3), 280–296. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.67.3.280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maki, R. H., & Berry, S. L. (1984). Metacomprehension of text material. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10(4), 663–679. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.10.4.663.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marchant, H. G., III, Royer, J. M., & Greene, B. A. (1988). Superior reliability and validity for a new form of the Sentence Verification Technique for measuring comprehension. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 48(3), 827–834. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164488483032.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masson, M. E., & Rotello, C. M. (2009). Sources of bias in the Goodman-Kruskal gamma coefficient measure of association: Implications for studies of metacognitive processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(2), 509. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014876.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mazzoni, G., & Cornoldi, C. (1993). Strategies in study time allocation: Why is study time sometimes not effective? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(1), 47–60. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.122.1.47.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, D. S., Kintsch, E., Songer, N. B., & Kintsch, W. (1996). Are good texts always better? Interactions of text coherence, background knowledge, and levels of understanding in learning from text. Cognition and Instruction, 14(1), 1–43. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532690xci1401_1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mengelkamp, C., & Bannert, M. (2010). Accuracy of confidence judgments: Stability and generality in the learning process and predictive validity for learning outcome. Memory & Cognition, 38(4), 441–451. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.4.441.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, B. J. (1975). The organization of prose and its effects on memory (Vol. 1). Amsterdam: Elsevier (North-Holland Publishing Co.).

    Google Scholar 

  • Narciss, S., Koerndle, H., & Dresel, M. (2011). Self-evaluation accuracy and satisfaction with performance: Are there affective costs or benefits of positive self-evaluation bias? International Journal of Educational Research, 50(4), 230–240. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2011.08.004.

    Google Scholar 

  • Narciss, S., Koerndle, H., & Proske, A. (2013). Challenges of investigating metacognitive tool use and effects in (rich) web-based learning environments. In R. Azevedo & V. Aleven (Eds.), International handbook of metacognition and learning technologies (pp. 243–260). New York: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-5546-3_17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O. (1996). Consciousness and metacognition. American Psychologist, 51(2), 102–116. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.51.2.102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 26, 125–173. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60053-5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nietfeld, J. L., Enders, C. K., & Schraw, G. (2006). A Monte Carlo comparison of measures of relative and absolute monitoring accuracy. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 66(2), 258–271. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164404273945.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ozuru, Y., Dempsey, K., & McNamara, D. S. (2009). Prior knowledge, reading skill, and text cohesion in the comprehension of science texts. Learning and Instruction, 19(3), 228–242. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2008.04.003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ozuru, Y., Kurby, C. A., & McNamara, D. S. (2012). The effect of metacomprehension judgment task on comprehension monitoring and metacognitive accuracy. Metacognition and Learning, 7(2), 113–131. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-012-9087-y.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: a dual coding approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A., Yuille, J. C., & Madigan, S. A. (1968). Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76(1), 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patall, E. A. (2013). Constructing motivation through choice, interest, and interestingness. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(2), 522. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030307.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2002). Are performance predictions for text based on ease of processing? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(1), 69–80. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.28.1.69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richman, W. L., Kiesler, S., Weisband, S., & Drasgow, F. (1999). A meta-analytic study of social desirability distortion in computer-administered questionnaires, traditional questionnaires, and interviews. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84(5), 754–775.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ritchey, K., Schuster, J., & Allen, J. (2008). How the relationship between text and headings influences readers’ memory. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 33(4), 859–874. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2007.11.001.

    Google Scholar 

  • Royer, J. M., & Cable, G. W. (1976). Illustrations, analogies, and facilitative transfer in prose learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 68(2), 205. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.68.2.205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sadoski, M. (2001). Resolving the effects of concreteness on interest, comprehension, and learning important ideas from text. Educational Psychology Review, 13(3), 263–281. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1016675822931.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sadoski, M., Goetz, E. T., & Fritz, J. B. (1993). Impact of concreteness on comprehensibility, interest, and memory for text: Implications for dual coding theory and text design. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(2), 291. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.85.2.291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sadoski, M., Goetz, E. T., & Rodriguez, M. (2000). Engaging texts: Effects of concreteness on comprehensibility, interest, and recall in four text types. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(1), 85. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.92.1.85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sadoski, M., & Paivio, A. (2004). A dual coding theoretical model of reading. In R. B. Ruddell & N. J. Unrau (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes of reading (5th ed., pp. 1329–1362). Newark, NJ: International Reading Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sadoski, M., & Paivio, A. (2013). Imagery and text—A dual-coding theory of reading and writing (2nd ed.). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schallert, D. L. (1976). Improving memory for prose: The relationship between depth of processing and context. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15(6), 621–632.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schnotz, W. (2005). An integrated model of text and picture comprehension. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 49–69). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schnotz, W., & Bannert, M. (2003). Construction and interference in learning from multiple representation. Learning and Instruction, 13(2), 141–156. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4752(02)00017-8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schraw, G. (2009). A conceptual analysis of five measures of metacognitive monitoring. Metacognition and Learning, 4(1), 33–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schraw, G., Bruning, R., & Svoboda, C. (1995). Sources of situational interest. Journal of Literacy Research, 27(1), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1080/10862969509547866.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schraw, G., & Lehman, S. (2001). Situational interest: A review of the literature and directions for future research. Educational Psychology Review, 13(1), 23–52. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1009004801455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schüler, A., Arndt, J., & Scheiter, K. (2015). Processing multimedia material: Does integration of text and pictures result in a single or two interconnected mental representations? Learning and Instruction, 35, 62–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2014.09.005.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, M. N. K., & Flammer, A. (1981). Text structure and title-effects on comprehension and recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 61–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepherd, S. (1990). Some effects of pictures and titles on 78 year olds’ recall and comprehension of aurally presented stories. Unpublished M.Ed. dissertation, University of Manchester.

  • Shimada, H. (2016). Effects of components of educational materials on motivation for reading. Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology, 64(3), 296–306. https://doi.org/10.5926/jjep.64.296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silvia, P. J. (2008). Interest—The curious emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(1), 57–60. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00548.x.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stone, N. J. (2000). Exploring the relationship between calibration and self-regulated learning. Educational Psychology Review, 12(4), 437–475. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1009084430926.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate statistics. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thiede, K. W., Anderson, M., & Therriault, D. (2003). Accuracy of metacognitive monitoring affects learning of texts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(1), 66–73. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thiede, K. W., Wiley, J., & Griffin, T. D. (2011). Test expectancy affects metacomprehension accuracy. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(2), 264–273. https://doi.org/10.1348/135910710X510494.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wade, S. E., Schraw, G., Buxton, W. M., & Hayes, M. T. (1993). Seduction of the strategic reader: Effects of interest on strategies and recall. Reading Research Quarterly, 28(2), 93–114. https://doi.org/10.2307/747885.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, J., Conder, J. A., Blitzer, D. N., & Shinkavera, S. V. (2010). Neural representation of abstract and concrete concepts: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 31(10), 1459–1468.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weaver, C. A. (1990). Constraining factors in calibration of comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(2), 214–222. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.16.2.214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiley, J., Griffin, T. D., & Thiede, K. W. (2005). Putting the comprehension in metacomprehension. The Journal of General Psychology, 132(4), 408–428. https://doi.org/10.3200/GENP.132.4.408-428.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winne, P. H., & Hadwin, A. F. (1998). Studying as self-regulated learning. In D. J. Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Metacognition in educational theory and practice (pp. 27–30). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yuill, N., & Joscelyn, T. (1988). Effect of organizational cues and strategies on good and poor comprehenders’ story understanding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(2), 152–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J., & Schunk, D. H. (2001). Self-regulated learning and academic achievement: Theoretical perspectives (2nd ed.). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marie Lippmann.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lippmann, M., Schwartz, N.H., Jacobson, N.G. et al. The concreteness of titles affects metacognition and study motivation. Instr Sci 47, 257–277 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-018-9478-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-018-9478-9

Keywords

Navigation