Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Residential Mobility Decreases the Perception of Social Norm Violations

  • Original Research
  • Published:
Social Indicators Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Social norms are essential but vary across human societies. With the internationalization of human society, the population’s mobility has greatly increased, which can impact people’s psychological states and behaviors. The current research aimed to examine the hypothesis that residential mobility plays a crucial role in the perception of social norm violations with six studies. Studies 1 and 2 used an association test and experimental manipulation, respectively, and found that residential mobility was associated with a decreased perception of weak social norm violations in females. Study 3 further suggested that residential mobility modulates individuals’ perception threshold to social norm violation behavior. Studies 4 and 5 revealed that the relationship between residential mobility and perception of social norm violations is mediated by face threats, and a mini meta-analysis further confirmed the significant effect of residential mobility on the perception of social norm violations. Our findings provide insights into how and why individuals’ perceptions of social norm-violating behaviors vary according to the dynamic development of society. As residential mobility continues to increase worldwide, especially in developing countries, we may observe concomitant changes in the subjective perception of social norms that should be given more attention during social governance.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abell, E., & Gecas, V. (1997). Guilt, shame, and family socialization: A retrospective study. Journal of Family Issues,18(2), 99–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aiken, L. S., West, S. G., & Reno, R. R. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Sage.

  • Bao, Y., Zhou, K. Z., & Su, C. (2003). Face consciousness and risk aversion: do they affect consumer decision-making? Psychology & Marketing,20(8), 733–755.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F. (1986). Identity: Cultural change and the struggle for self. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F. (1987). How the self became a problem: A psychological review of historical research. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,52(1), 163–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bicchieri, C. (2005). The grammar of society: The nature and dynamics of social norms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bosker, M., Brakman, S., Garretsen, H., & Schramm, M. (2012). Relaxing hukou: Increased labor mobility and china’ s economic geography. Journal of Urban Economics,72(2–3), 252–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1992). Punishment allows the evolution of cooperation (or anything else) in sizable groups. Ethology and Sociobiology,13(3), 171–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (1987). Politeness: Some universals in language usage. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Capraro, V., & Perc, M. (2018). Grand challenges in social physics: In pursuit of moral behavior. Frontiers in Physics, 6, 107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carter, J. D., & Hall, J. A. (2008). Individual differences in the accuracy of detecting social covariations: Ecological sensitivity. Journal of Research in Personality,42(2), 439–455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Castles, S., de Haas, H., & Miller, M. (2013). The age of migration-international movements in the modern world (S. Castles Ed. ed.).

  • Chan, H., Wan, L. C., & Sin, L. Y. (2009). The contrasting effects of culture on consumer tolerance: Interpersonal face and impersonal fate. Journal of Consumer Research,36(2), 292–304.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chekroun, P., & Brauer, M. (2002). The bystander effect and social control behavior: The effect of the presence of others on people’s reactions to norm violations. European Journal of Social Psychology,32(6), 853–867.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, X., Szolnoki, A., & Perc, M. (2012). Risk-driven migration and the collective-risk social dilemma. Physical Review E,86(3), 036101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chu, G. C. (1985). The changing concept of self in contemporary China. In A. J. Marsella, G. DeVos, & F. L. K. Hsu (Eds.), Culture and self: Asian and Western perspectives (pp. 252–277). New York: Tavistock Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chu, R. (2006). Social interactions among the Chinese: On the issue of face. Chinese Social Psychological Review,2, 79–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, D. (1998). Culture, social organization, and patterns of violence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,75(2), 408–419.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, D. (2001). Cultural variation: Considerations and implications. Psychological Bulletin,127(4), 451–471.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cross, S. E., & Madson, L. (1997). Models of the self: Self-construals and gender. Psychological Bulletin,122(1), 5–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Czaika, M., & De Haas, H. (2014). The globalization of migration: Has the world become more migratory? International Migration Review,48(2), 283–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ding, G., & Bao, Y. (2014). Editorial perspective: Assessing developmental risk in cultural context: the case of ‘left behind’ children in rural China. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,55(4), 411–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fan, C. C. (2003). Rural-urban migration and gender division of labor in transitional China. International Journal of Urban and Regional Research,27(1), 24–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelfand, M. J., Raver, J. L., Nishii, L., Leslie, L. M., Lun, J., Lim, B. C., et al. (2011). Differences between tight and loose cultures: A 33-nation study. Science,332(6033), 1100–1104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. New York: Garden City.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1967). Interaction ritual: Essays on face-to-face interaction. Oxford: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goh, J. X., Hall, J. A., & Rosenthal, R. (2016). Mini meta-analysis of your own studies: Some arguments on why and a primer on how. Social and Personality Psychology Compass,10(10), 535–549.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, I., Vander Wyk, B. C., Bennett, R. H., Cordeaux, C., Lucas, M. V., Eilbott, J. A., et al. (2013). Oxytocin enhances brain function in children with autism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,110(52), 20953–20958.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guimond, S., Chatard, A., Martinot, D., Crisp, R. J., & Redersdorff, S. (2006). Social comparison, self-stereotyping, and gender differences in self-construals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,90(2), 221–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halpern, D. (2001). Moral values, social trust and inequality: Can values explain crime? British Journal of Criminology,41(2), 236–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helbing, D., Brockmann, D., Chadefaux, T., Donnay, K., Blanke, U., Woolley-Meza, O., et al. (2015). Saving human lives: What complexity science and information systems can contribute. Journal of Statistical Physics,158(3), 735–781.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & Boyd, R. (2001). Why people punish defectors: Weak conformist transmission can stabilize costly enforcement of norms in cooperative dilemmas. Journal of Theoretical Biology,208(1), 79–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ho, D. (1991). The concept of “face” in Chinese-American interaction. In Encountering the Chinese: a guide for Americans, 111–124.

  • Ho, V. T., Rousseau, D. M., & Levesque, L. L. (2006). Social networks and the psychological contract: Structural holes, cohesive ties, and beliefs regarding employer obligations. Human Relations,59(4), 459–481.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holleran, P. A., Littman, D. C., Freund, R. D., & Schmaling, K. B. (1982). A signal detection approach to social perception: Identification of negative and positive behaviors by parents of normal and problem children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,10(4), 547–557.

    Google Scholar 

  • International Organization for Migration. (2015). World migration 2015: Migrants and cities: New partnerships to manage mobility. Retrieved from Geneva, Switzerland: Author.

  • Jonsson, G. (2009). Comparative report: African migration trends. African perspectives on human mobility programme.

  • Joy, A. (2001). Gift giving in Hong Kong and the continuum of social ties. Journal of Consumer Research,28(2), 239–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • King, A. (1988). Face, shame and the analysis of behavior patterns of the Chinese. In The Psychology of the Chinese, pp. 319–345.

  • King, A., & Bond, M. H. (1985). The Confucian paradigm of man: A sociological view. In Chinese culture and mental health, pp. 29–45.

  • Krumhuber, E. G., Tsankova, E., & Kappas, A. (2016). Examining subjective and physiological responses to norm violation using text-based vignettes. International Journal of Psychology,53(1), 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, M. D., & Wagenmakers, E. J. (2014). Bayesian cognitive modeling: A practical course. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levari, D. E., Gilbert, D. T., Wilson, T. D., Beau, S., Amodio, D. M., & Thalia, W. (2018). Prevalence-induced concept change in human judgment. Science,360(6396), 1465–1467.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, J. J., & Su, C. (2007). How face influences consumption. International Journal of Market Research,49(2), 237–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lu, M., & Gao, H. (2011). Labour market transition, income inequality and economic growth in China. International Labour Review, 150, 1–2.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lun, J., Oishi, S., & Tenney, E. R. (2012). Residential mobility moderates preferences for egalitarian versus loyal helpers. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,48(1), 291–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ma, Y., & Han, S. (2012). Functional dissociation of the left and right fusiform gyrus in self-face recognition. Human Brain Mapping,33(10), 2255–2267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Magdol, L. (2002). Is moving gendered? The effects of residential mobility on the psychological well-being of men and women. Sex roles,47(11), 553–560.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mak, W. W., Chen, S. X., Lam, A. G., & Yiu, V. F. (2009). Understanding distress: the role of face concern among Chinese Americans, European Americans, Hong Kong Chinese, and mainland Chinese. The Counseling Psychologist,37(2), 219–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mak, W. W., & Cheung, R. Y. (2012). Psychological distress and subjective burden of caregivers of people with mental illness: the role of affiliate stigma and face concern. Community Mental Health Journal,48(3), 270–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review,98(2), 224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mazzurega, M., Marisa, J., Zampini, M., & Pavani, F. (2018). Thinner than yourself: Self-serving bias in body size estimation. Psychological Research. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1119-z.

  • McElreath, R. (2004). Community structure, mobility and the strength of norms in an African society, the Sangu of Tanzania. In Foundations of human sociality: Ethnogra, pp. 335–355.

  • Miller, S. C., Kennedy, C. C., DeVoe, D. C., Hickey, M., Nelson, T., & Kogan, L. (2009). An examination of changes in oxytocin levels in men and women before and after interaction with a bonded dog. Anthrozoös,22(1), 31–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moineddin, R., Matheson, F. I., & Glazier, R. H. (2007). A simulation study of sample size for multilevel logistic regression models. BMC Medical Research Methodology,7(1), 34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mu, Y., Kitayama, S., Han, S., & Gelfand, M. J. (2015). How culture gets embrained: Cultural differences in event-related potentials of social norm violations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,112(50), 15348–15353.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oetzel, J., Ting-Toomey, S., Masumoto, T., Yokochi, Y., Pan, X., Takai, J., et al. (2001). Face and facework in conflict: A cross-cultural comparison of China, Germany, Japan, and the United States. Communication Monographs,68(3), 235–258.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ohbuchi, K. I., Tamura, T., Quigley, B. M., Tedeschi, J. T., Madi, N., Bond, M. H., et al. (2004). Anger, blame, and dimensions of perceived norm violations: Culture, gender, and relationships. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,34(8), 1587–1603.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oishi, S. (2010). The psychology of residential mobility implications for the self, social relationships, and well-being. Perspectives on Psychological Science,5(1), 5–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oishi, S., & Graham, J. (2010). Social ecology: Lost and found in psychological science. Perspectives on Psychological Science,5(4), 356–377.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oishi, S., Kesebir, S., Miao, F. F., Talhelm, T., Endo, Y., Uchida, Y., et al. (2013). Residential mobility increases motivation to expand social network: But why? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,49(2), 217–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oishi, S., Lun, J., & Sherman, G. D. (2007). Residential mobility, self-concept, and positive affect in social interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,93(1), 131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oishi, S., Miao, F. F., Koo, M., Kisling, J., & Ratliff, K. A. (2012). Residential mobility breeds familiarity-seeking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,102(1), 149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oishi, S., & Talhelm, T. (2012). Residential mobility: What psychological research reveals. Current Directions in Psychological Science,21(6), 425–430.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ozsoy, S., Esel, E., & Kula, M. (2009). Serum oxytocin levels in patients with depression and the effects of gender and antidepressant treatment. Psychiatry Research,169(3), 249–252.

    Google Scholar 

  • Royzman, E. B., Leeman, R. F., & Baron, J. (2009). Unsentimental ethics: Towards a content-specific account of the moral–conventional distinction. Cognition,112(1), 159–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sampson, R. J., Raudenbush, S. W., & Earls, F. (1997). Neighborhoods and violent crime: a multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science,277(5328), 918–924.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoemann, A. M., Boulton, A. J., & Short, S. D. (2017). Determining power and sample size for simple and complex mediation models. Social Psychological and Personality Science,8, 379–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Su, J. C., Chiu, C. Y., Lin, W. F., & Oishi, S. (2016). Social monitoring matters for deterring social deviance in stable but not mobile socio-ecological contexts. PLoS ONE,11(11), e0167053.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thornton, I. M., Srismith, D., Oxner, M., & Hayward, W. G. (2019). Other-race faces are given more weight than own-race faces when assessing the composition of crowds. Vision Research, 157, 159–168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ting-Toomey, S., Gao, G., Trubisky, P., Yang, Z., Soo Kim, H., Lin, S.-L., et al. (1991). Culture, face maintenance, and styles of handling interpersonal conflict: A study in five cultures. International Journal of conflict management,2(4), 275–296.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Honk, J., Schutter, D. J., Hermans, E. J., Putman, P., Tuiten, A., & Koppeschaar, H. (2004). Testosterone shifts the balance between sensitivity for punishment and reward in healthy young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology,29(7), 937–943.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, C. S., & Leung, A. K. Y. (2010). The cultural dynamics of rewarding honesty and punishing deception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,36(11), 1529–1542.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2012). Biosocial construction of sex differeces and similarities in behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology,46, 55–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wu, W. (2006). Migrant intra-urban residential mobility in urban China. Housing Studies,21(5), 745–765.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wu, T., Fu, F., Zhang, Y., & Wang, L. (2012). Expectation-driven migration promotes cooperation by group interactions. Physical Review E,85(6), 066104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yang, K. S. (1981). Social orientation and individual modernity among Chinese students in Taiwan. Journal of Social Psychology,113(2), 159–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yu, J., & Xie, Y. (2015). Cohabitation in China: Trends and determinants. Population and Development Review, 41(4), 607–628.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by grants from the Science Foundation of Ministry of Education of China (Project 17YJCZH121) and the Natural Science Foundation of Guangdong Province (Project 2017A030310553) and the Natural Science Foundation of China (Project 31800916).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

KQT and LSY designed the research; KQT, ZYY, XY conducted the experiment; KQT, KZJ and LSY analyzed the data; and KQT, YMH, HLQ, LSY and KZJ wrote the manuscript. All authors commented on the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Siyang Luo.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Ethics Statement

All participants gave written informed consent, and the study was approved by the Department of Psychology of Sun Yat-sen University Ethics Committee.

Code Availability

The custom computer code and questionnaire that was used in the main analysis of this study is available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Data Availability

The deidentified data that support the main findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 439 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Luo, S., Kong, Q., Ke, Z. et al. Residential Mobility Decreases the Perception of Social Norm Violations. Soc Indic Res 148, 961–986 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-019-02224-7

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-019-02224-7

Keywords

Navigation