Skip to main content
Log in

Who Counts as Human? Antecedents to Androcentric Behavior

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Sex Roles Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

People view men as typically human, although some conditions may make this more or less likely. Language has been implicated as one factor, with masculine generic language (e.g., he used neutrally) leading to more androcentrism relative to its alternatives. However, the influence of two types of alternatives (e.g., they vs. he or she) remains unclear. The present study asked 297 male and female online participants from the United States to select typical representations of humanity from a set of White and Black male and female faces. The wording for the concept humanity was manipulated to be either a typical member of mankind, a typical human, or a typical man or woman (or woman or man). Overall, participants selected more White targets. Participants also selected more male targets, but the degree to which that was the case was affected by wording and participant’s gender. Participants, particularly male participants, in the mankind and human wording conditions were more likely to select a male target as representative, whereas in the man or woman condition, participants’ choices did not differ from chance. Thus, androcentric thinking may be more mutable than previously surmised, varying by participants’ gender and by context.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • American Psychological Association Publication Manual Task Force (1978). Guidelines for non-sexist language in APA journals: Publication manual change sheet 2. Educational Researcher, 7(3), 487–494.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beery, T. A. (1995). Gender bias in the diagnosis and treatment of coronary artery disease. Heart & Lung: The Journal of Acute and Critical Care, 24(6), 427–435. doi:10.1016/S0147-9563(95)80020-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bem, S. L. (1993). The lenses of gender: Transforming the debate on sexual inequality. New Haven, CT: Yale University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bem, S. L. (1995). Dismantling gender polarization and compulsory heterosexuality: Should we turn the volume down or up? The Journal of Sex Research, 32(4), 329–334. doi:10.1080/00224499509551806.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bodine, A. (1975). Androcentrism in prescriptive grammar: Singular ‘they,’ sex-indefinite ‘he,’ and ‘he or she.’. Language in Society, 4(2), 129–146. doi:10.1017/S0047404500004607.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, R. R. W., & Purdie-Vaughns, V. (2007). Supermodular architecture of inclusion. Harvard Journal of Law and Gender, 30, 379–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broverman, I. K., Broverman, D. M., Clarkson, F. E., Rosenkrantz, P. S., & Vogel, S. R. (1970). Sex-role stereotypes and clinical judgements of mental health. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 34(1), 1–7. doi:10.1037/h0028797.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burke, S. E., & LaFrance, M. (2015). Lay conceptions of sexual minority groups. Archives of Sexual Behavior. Advance online publication. doi:10.1007/s10508-015-0655-5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Center for American Women and Politics. (2013). Women in the U.S. Congress 2013. Retrieved from http://www.cawp.rutgers.edu/fast_facts/levels_of_office/documents/cong.pdf.

  • Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155–159. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crosby, F., Clayton, S., Alksnis, O., & Hemker, K. (1986). Cognitive biases in the perception of discrimination: The importance of format. Sex Roles, 14(11/12), 637–646. doi:10.1007/BF00287694.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cundiff, J. L. (2012). Is mainstream psychological research womanless and raceless? An updated analysis. Sex Roles, 67(3–4), 158–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Beauvoir, S. (2010). The second sex. (Trans: C. Borde & S. Malovany-Chevallier). New York: Vintage Books. (Original work published 1949).

  • Eagly, A. H., & Kite, M. E. (1987). Are stereotypes of nationalities applied to both women and men? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(3), 451–462. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.53.3.451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., Wood, W., & Diekman, A. B. (2000). Social role theory of sex differences and similarities: A current appraisal. In T. Eckes & H. M. Trautner (Eds.), The developmental social psychology of gender (pp. 123–174). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. doi:10.3758/BF03193146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fausto-Sterling, A. (1993). The five sexes. The Sciences, 33(2), 20–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., & Stevens, L. E. (1993). What's so special about sex? Gender stereotyping and discrimination. In S. Oskamp & M. Costanzo (Eds.), Gender issues in contemporary society: Applied social psychology annual (pp. 173–196). Newbury Park: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gastil, J. (1990). Generic pronouns and sexist language: The oxymoronic character of masculine generics. Sex Roles, 23(11), 629–643. doi:10.1007/BF00289252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ginther, D. K., & Kahn, S. (2009). Does science promote women? Evidence from academia 1973–2001. In R. B. Freeman & D. F. Goroff (Eds.), Science and engineering careers in the United States: An analysis of markets and employment (pp. 163–194). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • GLAAD. (2015). Tips for allies of transgender people. Retrieved from http://www.glaad.org/transgender/allies.

  • Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (2001). An ambivalent alliance: Hostile and benevolent sexism as complementary justifications for gender inequality. American Psychologist, 56(2), 109–118.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grant, J. M., Mottet, L., Tanis, J. E., Harrison, J., Herman, J., & Keisling, M. (2011). Injustice at every turn: A report of the national transgender discrimination survey. Washington, DC: National Center for Transgender Equality.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, M. C. (1991). Masculine bias in the attribution of personhood: People = male, male = people. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 15, 393–402. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1991.tb00415.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hegarty, P., & Buechel, C. (2006). Androcentric reporting of gender differences in APA journals: 1964-2004. Review of General Psychology, 10(4), 377–389. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.10.4.377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, E., & Agnoli, F. (1991). The Whorfian hypothesis: A cognitive psychology perspective. Psychological Review, 98(3), 377–389. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.98.3.377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, J. S. (1984). Children’s understanding of sexist language. Developmental Psychology, 20(4), 697–706. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.20.4.697.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Miller, D. T. (1986). Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives. Psychological Review, 93(2), 136–153. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.93.2.136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lambdin, J. R., Greer, K. M., Jibotian, K. S., Wood, K. R., & Hamilton, M. C. (2003). The animal = male hypothesis: Children’s and adults’ beliefs about the sex of non-sex-specific stuffed animals. Sex Roles, 48(11/12), 471–482. doi:10.1023/A:1023567010708.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LGBTQ Resource Center. (2015). Gender pronouns. Retrieved from https://uwm.edu/lgbtrc/support/gender-pronouns/.

  • Linville, P. W., & Fischer, G. W. (1993). Exemplar and abstraction models of perceived group variability and stereotypicality. Social Cognition, 11(1), 92–125. doi:10.1521/soco.1993.11.1.92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ma, D. S., Correll, J., & Wittenbrink, B. (2015). The Chicago face database: A free stimulus set of faces and norming data. Behavior Research Methods, 47, 1122–1135. doi:10.3758/s13428-014-0532-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Madson, L., & Hessling, R. M. (1999). Does alternating between masculine and feminine pronouns eliminate perceived gender bias in text? Sex Roles, 41(7/8), 559–575. doi:10.1023/A:1018895321444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, M. M., & James, L. E. (2009). Is the generic pronoun he still comprehended as excluding women? The American Journal of Psychology, 122(4), 483–496.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, D. T., Taylor, B., & Buck, M. L. (1991). Gender gaps: Who needs to be explained? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(1), 15–21. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.61.1.5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moss-Racusin, C. A., Dovidio, J. F., Brescoll, V. L., Graham, M. J., & Handelsman, J. (2012). Science faculty’s subtle gender biases favor male students. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(41), 16474–16479. doi:10.1073/pnas.1211286109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moulton, J., Robinson, G. M., & Elias, C. (1978). Psychology in action: sex bias in language use: “Neutral” pronouns that aren’t. American Psychologist, 33, 1032–1036. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.33.11.1032.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ng, S. H. (1989). Androcentric coding of man and his in memory by language users. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 26, 455–464. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(90)90069-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prentice, D. A. (1994). Do language reforms change our way of thinking? Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 13, 3–19. doi:10.1177/0261927X94131001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, J. W., & Hacker, S. L. (1973). Sex role imagery and use of the generic “man” in introductory texts: A case in the sociology of sociology. The American Sociologist, 8(1), 12–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silveira, J. (1980). Generic masculine words and thinking. Women’s Studies International Quarterly, 3, 165–178. doi:10.1016/S0148-0685(80)92113-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. R., & Zárate, M. A. (1992). Exemplar-based model of social judgment. Psychological Review, 99(1), 3–21. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.99.1.3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stangor, C., Lynch, L., Duan, C., & Glas, B. (1992). Categorization of individuals on the basis of multiple social features. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(2), 207–218. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.62.2.207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stroessner, S. J. (1996). Social categorization by race or sex: Effects of perceived non-normalcy on response times. Social Cognition, 14(3), 247–276. doi:10.1521/soco.1996.14.3.247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Switzer, J. Y. (1990). The impact of generic word choices: An empirical investigation of age- and sex-related differences. Sex Roles, 22(1/2), 69–81. doi:10.1007/BF00288155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33–47). Monterey: Brooks/Cole.

    Google Scholar 

  • Twenge, J. M. (2012). Male and female pronoun use in U.S. books reflects women’s status, 1900-2008. Sex Roles, 69(9–10), 488–493. doi:10.1007/s11199-012-0194-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uddenberg, S., & Scholl, B. (2015). Revealing mental defaults in face space with serial reproduction. Journal of Vision, 15(12), 1214–1214. doi:10.1167/15.12.1214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • United States Department of Labor. (2015). Women in the labor force. Retrieved from http://www.dol.gov/wb/stats/facts_over_time.htm#content.

  • Williams, W. M., & Ceci, S. J. (2015). National hiring experiments reveal 2:1 faculty preference for women on STEM tenure track. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(17), 5360–5365. doi:10.1073/pnas.1418878112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winter, J., & Pauwels, A. (2006). Men staying at home looking after their children: Feminist linguistic reform and social change. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 16(1), 16–36. doi:10.1111/j.1473-4192.2006.00104.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wohl, M. J. A., & Branscombe, N. R. (2005). Forgiveness and collective guilt assignment to historical perpetrator groups depend on level of social category inclusiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(2), 288–303. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88.2.288.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zárate, M. A., & Smith, E. R. (1990). Person categorization and stereotyping. Social Cognition, 8(2), 161–185. doi:10.1521/soco.1990.8.2.161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge the Yale University Psychology Department for providing funding for the project.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to April H. Bailey.

Ethics declarations

The present research involves human subjects; as detailed in the manuscript, the research was reviewed and approved by the Yale Human Subjects Committee.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Electronic Supplementary Material

ESM 1

(DOCX 13 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bailey, A.H., LaFrance, M. Who Counts as Human? Antecedents to Androcentric Behavior. Sex Roles 76, 682–693 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0648-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0648-4

Keywords

Navigation