Skip to main content
Log in

Abstract

The nature of concepts is a subject of study of various disciplines, from philosophy to cognitive sciences, leading to fragmented understandings and conceptual dissociations. Legal concepts have been studied in an interdisciplinary manner across all these disciplines, suffering from similar fragmentation. Recently, the interdisciplinary crossroads between law and cognitive sciences have brought forward the notion of legal concepts as mental representations. However, this approach largely overlooks the systemic, historical, and societal elements essential to comprehending legal concepts. The aim of this paper is to advocate for the Social Representations Theory as a useful framework that bridges cognitive and socio-cultural dimensions of meaning and can provide a holistic approach to understanding legal concepts. This paper unfolds in three sections. The first section contextualizes the social representations approach within the law and language framework, emphasizing the societal influences on thought and meaning. The second section explains the notion of social representations, building upon Serge Moscovici’s definitions and Ivana Marková’s arguments for the necessity of this approach to accommodate the social dimension of meaning. The third and last section underscores the claim that legal concepts are, in essence, social representations, advocating for the usefulness of this approach in legal scholarship, both paradigmatically and methodologically, consequently arguing for an inclusion for a stronger focus on the social dimension of legal meaning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Compare Marková [38, p. 1] in the context of researchers’ presuppositions influencing their research.

  2. Literature sometimes speaks of ‘representing an object’ [64, p. 96], sometimes equalling the object to its representation (see further discussion in this text. See also [64, p. 114; 65, p. 169; 74, p. 21].

  3. The notion of common sense may seem to create a strong link between the social representations approach and folk psychology. Folk psychology is used to denote the theories humans use to predict behaviour of others within an everyday way of understanding and rationalizing [72] or ‘a set of beliefs and practices about psychological issues in a particular culture’ [73]. While both social representations and folk psychology are cognitive frameworks used by individuals to navigate social world, they refer to fundamentally distinct theories: while social representations focus on the social meaning, folk psychology is concerned individual-level cognitive processes.

  4. It is not the purpose of this paper to tackle these further, suffice to say that the core/periphery thinking goes across linguistic, philosophical and cognitive studies. See for example [111,112,113].

  5. Calling the core as normative implies that core elements of social representations make it what it is and what it does within a given social group. To be understood, one needs to be able to reach for the core elements. The communicative processes of social representation formation are social in nature, similarly as various other processes, such as norm formation in terms of traditions, or customary law. Shared knowledge has a strong normative and dynamic nature. Social majorities (or individuals and groups with a strong social capital) may push the change of meaning, i.e. social representations [114]. To fully tackle the relationship between social representation construction and norm formation in terms of customary law is out the scope of this paper.

  6. It is out of scope of this paper to delve into the discussion on cognitive universalia.

  7. Hermeneutics brings the concept of preunderstanding into consideration at this point. To delve into this particular dimension would be outside the scope of this paper.

References

  1. Lamberts, K., and D. Shanks. 1997. Knowledge, Concepts, and Categories. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  2. Murphy, G.L. 2002. The Big Book of Concepts. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  3. von der Pfordten, D. 2009. About Concepts in Law. In Concepts in Law, ed. J. Hage and D. von der Pfordten, 35–54. Dodrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Hjørland, B. 2009. Concept Theory. Journal ofthe American Society for Information Science and Technology 60 (8): 1519–1536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. de Saussure, F. 2007. Kurs obecné lingvistiky. Praha: Academia.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Ogden, C.K., and I.A. Richards. 1930. The Meaning of Meaning. A Study of the Influence of Language upon Thought and of the Science of Symbolism. London: Trubner & Co. Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Materna, P. 2000. Svět pojmů a logika. Praha: Filosofia.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Frege, G. 1948. Sense and Reference. The Philosophical Review 57 (3): 209–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Fodor, J. 2003. Hume Variations. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Fodor, J. 2008. LOT 2. The Language of Thought Revisited. Oxford: Claredon Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  11. Laurence, S., and E. Margolis. 1999. Concepts and Cognitive Science. In Concepts: Core Readings, ed. Margolis and S. Laurence, 3–81. Cambridge: Bradford Book.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Pinker, S. 2007. The Stuff of Thought: Language as a Window into Human Nature. London: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Cassanto, D., and G. Lupyan. 2015. All Concepts are Ad Hoc Concepts. In The Conceptual Mind: New Directions in the Study of Concepts, ed. S. Laurence and E. Margolis, 543–566. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  14. Hofmann, H. 2009. From Jhering to Radbruch: On the Logic of Traditional Legal Concepts to the Social Theories of Law to the Renewal of Legal Idealism. In

  15. Glogar, O. 2023. The Concept of Legal Language: What Makes Legal Language ‘Legal’? International Journal for the Semiotics of Law 36 (3): 1081–1107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Tiersma, P. 1999. Legal Language. Chicago: UCP.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Mellinkoff, D. 2004. The Language of the Law. Eugene: Resource Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Bittar, E.C.B. 2022. Semiotics of Law, Science of Law and Legal Meaning: Analysis of the Status of Legal Dogmatics. Signata. https://doi.org/10.4000/signata.4129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Wagner, Anne, King Kui Sin, and Le. Cheng. 2014. Cultural Transfer and Conceptualization in Legal Discourse. In The Ashgate Handbook of Legal Translation, ed. Le. Cheng, King Kui Sin, and Anne Wagner. Abingdon: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Lloyd, H.A. 2021. How to do Things With Signs: Semiotics in Legal Theory, Practice, and Education. University of Richmond Law Review 55: 861. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3516238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Brandom, R.B. 2014. A Hegelian Model of Legal Concept Determination: The Normative Fine Structure of the Judge’s Chain Novel. In Pragmatism, Law, and Language, ed. G. Hubbs and D. Lind, 19–39. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Wittgenstein, L. 1958. Philosophical Investigations. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Hamilton, J. 2002. Theories of Categorization: A Case Study of Cheques. Canadian Journal of Law and Society 17 (1): 115–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Allison, Neil Grainger. 2023. From Semantic Weight to Legal Ontology via Classification of Concepts in Legal Texts. The Law Teacher 57 (2): 201–217. https://doi.org/10.1080/03069400.2023.2173918.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Vecellio Segate, R. 2022. Shifting Privacy Rights from the Individual to the Group: A Re-adaptation of Algorithms Regulation to Address the Gestaltian Configuration of Groups. Loyola Univesity of Chicago Journal of Regulatory Compliance, Issue VIII. https://www.compliancelawjournal.com/compliancelawjournal/issue_viii/MobilePagedArticle.action?articleId=1793260

  26. Zeifert, M. 2020. Prototype Theory in the Judicial Practice of the Court of Justice of the European Union. A Case Study. Comparative Legilinguistics 44: 93–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Fränberg, A. 2009. An Essay on Legal Concept Formation. In Concepts in Law, ed. J. Hage and D. von der Pfordten, 35–54. Dodrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Jakubiec, M. 2022. Legal Concepts as Mental Representations. International Journal for the Semiotics of Law 35: 1837–1855.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Margolis, E. 2004. A reassessment of the Shift from the Classical Theory of Concepts to Prototype Theory. Cognition 51: 73–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Margolis, E., and S. Laurence. 2021. Concepts. The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Spring 2021 Edition). In ed. Edward N. Zalta. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2021/entries/concepts/ Accessed 25 May 2022.

  31. Smejkalová, T. 2022. Case Law and Collective Construction of Meaning. Utrecht Law Review. https://doi.org/10.36633/ulr.833.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Rosa, De., Annamaria Silvana, and Arhiri Laura. 2020. The Anthropological and Ethnographic Approaches to Social Reperesentations Theory—An Empirical Meta-Theoretical Analysis. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-020-09559-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Moscovici, S. 1961/1976. La psychanalyse son image et son public. Etude sur la répresentation sociale de la psychanalyse. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

  34. Liu, X., X. Zhao, and B. Wang. 2022. Analysis of Personal Psychological Motivation and Social Psychological Motivation of Retaliatory Justice Under Moral Public Opinion: Chinese Cases. Frontiers in Psychology 13: 1021577. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1021577.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Spruill, M., and N.A. Lewis. 2023. How Do People Come to Judge What Is “Reasonable”? Effects of Legal and Sociological Systems on Human Psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science 18 (2): 378–391. https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916221096110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Jovchelovitch, S. 1995. Social Representations in and of the Public Sphere: Towards a Theoretical Articulation. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 25 (1): 81–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Villani, C., S. D’Ascenzo, A.M. Borghi, C. Roversi, M. Benassi, and L. Lugli. 2022. Is Justice Grounded? How Expertise Shapes Conceptual Representation of Institutional Concepts. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung 86 (8): 2434–2450. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-021-01492-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Marková, Ivana. 2003. Dialogicality and Social Representations: The Dynamics of Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Moscovici, S. 1994. Social Representations and Pragmatic Communication. Social Science Information 2: 163–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Marková, I. 2007. Dialogičnost a Sociální Reprezentace: Dynamika Mysli. Praha: Academia.

    Google Scholar 

  41. White, J.B. 1985. The Legal Imagination. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Bhatia, V.K. 2008. Legal Discourse Across Cultures and Systems. Hong Kong University Press. https://doi.org/10.5790/hongkong/9789622098510.001.0001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. White, J.B. 1981–1982. Law as Language: Reading Law and Reading Literature. Texas Law Review 3: 415–445.

  44. Mattila, H.E. 2006. Comparative Legal Linguistics. Aldershot: Ashgate Publishing Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Goodrich, P. 1984. Law and Language: An Historical and Critical Introduction. Journal of Law and Society 2: 173–206. https://doi.org/10.2307/1410039.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Fish, S.E.E. 1976. Interpreting the “Variorum.” Critical Inquiry 3: 465–485. https://doi.org/10.1086/447852.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Fish, S.E.E. 1980. Is There a Text in This Class?: The Authority of Interpretive Communities. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Smejkalová, T., and M. Štěpáníková. 2019. Law for Elites. Studies in Logic, Grammar and Rhetoric 1: 47–68. https://doi.org/10.2478/slgr-2019-0028.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Wenger, E. 1998. Communities of Practice: Learning, Meaning, and Identity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511803932.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  50. Yule, G. 2017. The Study of Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Morgan, M.H. 2014. Speech Communities. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  52. Labov, W. 1972. Sociolinguistic Patterns. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Chaib, M., et al. 2011. Introduction: Social Knowledge—Shared, Transmitted, Transformed. In Education, Professionalization and Social Representations, ed. M. Chaib, et al., 1–16. London: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203837207.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  54. Dodds, A.E., J.A. Lawrence, and J. Valsiner. 1997. The Personal and the Social Mead’s Theory of the ‘Generalized Other.’ Theory & Psychology 7 (4): 483–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Potter, J. 1996. Representing Reality. Discourse, Rhetoric and Social Construction. London: Sage Publication.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  56. Höijer, B. 2011. Social Representations Theory. A New Theory for Media Research. Nordicom Review 2: 3–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Smejkalová, T., et al. 2022. Veřejný pořádek, důstojnost soudce a judikatura. Brno: MUNIPress.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  58. Moscovici, S. 1984. The Phenomenon of Social Representations. In Social Representations, ed. R.M. Farr and S. Moscovici, 21–28. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Moscovici, S. 1963. Attitudes and Opinions. Annual Review of Psychology 14: 231–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Wachelke, J. 2012. Social Representations: A Review of Theory and Research from the Structural Approach. Universitas Psychologica 11 (3): 729–741.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Flick, U., and J. Foster. 2008. Social Representations. In Handbook of Qualitative Research in Psychology, ed. C. Willig and W. Stainton-Rogers, 261–279. London: Sage Publications Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Marková, I. 1996. Towards an Epistemology of Social Representations. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 26 (2): 177–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Wachelke, J. 2013. Beyond Social Representations: The Conceptual Bases of the Structural Approach on Social Thinking. Revista Interamericana de Psicología/Interamerican Journal of Psychology 47 (1): 131–138.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Wagner, W. 1996. Queries About Social Representation and Construction. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 2: 95–120. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5914.1996.tb00524.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Bauer, M.W., and G. Gaskell. 1999. Towards a Paradigm for Research on Social Representations. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 29 (2): 163–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Codol, J.-P. 1969. Note Terminologique sur l’emploi de quelques Expressions Concernant les Activités et Processus cognitifs en Psychologie Sociale. Bulletin de Psychologie 23 (280): 63–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Locke, J. 1690/1975. An Essay Concerning Human Understanding. New York: Oxford University Press.

  68. Jodelet, D. 2021. The notion of common and social representations. In Serge Moscovici’s work, ed. Papastamou, Stamos, Moliner, Pascal. Legacy and perspective. Archives contemporaines, 87–98. https://doi.org/10.17184/eac.4968

  69. Fodor, J.A. 2000. The Mind Doesn’t Work That Way The Scope and Limits of Computational Psychology. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  70. Janko, T. 2012. Reprezentace obsahu: Psychologická východiska a didaktické souvislosti. Pedagogická Orientace 22 (1): 30–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Jaspars, J., and C. Fraser. 1984. Attitudes and Social Representations. In Social Representations, ed. R.M. Farr and S. Moscovici, 101–123. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  72. Hutto, D., and Ravenscroft, I. 2021. Folk Psychology as a Theory. In Edward N. Zalta (ed.) The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall 2021 Edition), URL = <https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2021/entries/folkpsych-theory/>.

  73. Nsamenang, A.B. 2001. Indigenous View on Human Development: West African Perspective. In International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, Pergamon, ed. Neil J. Smelser, Paul B. Baltes, 7297–7299.

  74. Moscovici, S. 2001. Social Representations. Explorations in Social Psychology. New York: New York University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  75. Moscovici, S. 1973. Preface. In Health and Illness, ed. C. Herzlich, V–VII. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  76. Wagner, W. 1995. Description, Exlanation and Method. Social Representation Research. Papers on Social Representations 4 (2): 1–176.

    Google Scholar 

  77. Durkheim, É. 1912/1995. The Elementary Forms of the Religious Life. New York: Free Press.

  78. Durkheim, É. 1957. Professional Ethics and Civil Morals. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  79. Turner, S.B. 1957. Preface to the 2nd Edition. In Professional Ethics and Civil, ed. Morals É Durkheim. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  80. Farr, R.M. 1998. From Collective to Social Representations: Aller et Retour. Culture and Psychology 4 (3): 275–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Lévy-Bruhl, L. 1966. How Natives Think. New York: Washington Square Pres.

    Google Scholar 

  82. Rippon, G. 2019. The Gendered Brain. The New Neuroscience that Shatters the Myth of the Female Brain. London: Vintage.

    Google Scholar 

  83. Wagner, W., et al. 1995. How Sperm Dominates the Ovum—Objectification by Metaphor in the Social Representation of Conception. European Journal of Social Psychology 15 (6): 671–688.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  84. Psaltis, C. 2015. Communication and the Microgenetic Construction of Knowledge. In The Cambridge Handbook of Social Representations, ed. G. Sammut, et al., 113–127. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107323650.011.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  85. Bourdieu, P. 1992. Language and Symbolic Power. Polity Press.

    Google Scholar 

  86. van Hoecke, M. 2002. Law as Communication. Oxford and Portland: Hart Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  87. Bourdieu, P. 1986–1987. The Force of Law: Toward a Sociology of the Juridical Field. Hastings L.J. 38: 814

  88. Bakhtin, M. 1981. The Dialogic Imagination. University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  89. Latour, Bruno. 2004. Why Has Critique Run out of Steam? From Matters of Fact to Matters of Concern. Ciritical Inquiry 30 (2): 225 ff.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Bex, Floris, Prakken, Henry. 2021. Can Predictive Justice Improve the Predictability and Consistency of Judicial Decision-Making?. In Legal Knowledge and Information Systems, ed. E. Schweighofer, 207–214. IOS Press. https://doi.org/10.3233/FAIA210338.

  91. Miller, G.P. 2005. The Legal Function of Ritual. Chicago-Kent Law Review 80(3): 1181.

    Google Scholar 

  92. Reid, A., L.O. Dalhgren, P. Petocz, and M.A. Dahlgren. 2008. Identitty and Engagement for Professional Formation. Studies in Higher Education 33 (6): 729–742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  93. Sommerlad, H. 2008. Researching and Theorizing the Processes of Professional Identity Formation. Journal of Law and Society 34 (2): 190–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  94. Andrade, P.C. de et al. 2022. What do We Mean by Precedent? Empirical Evidence of Ordinary Usage. Preprint. Researchgate. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/342354206_What_do_we_mean_by_precedent_Empirical_evidence_of_ordinary_ usage. Accessed 30 Sept 2022.

  95. Tobia, K. 2020. Testing Ordinary Meaning. Harward Law Review 134 (2): 726–806.

    Google Scholar 

  96. Chi, M.T.H. 1981. Categorization and Representation of Physics Problems by Experts and Novices. Cognitive Science A Multidisciplinary Journal 5 (2): 121–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  97. De Paolis, Paola. 1990/2005. Prototypes of the psychologist and professionalisation: diverging social representations of a developmental process. In Social Representations and Development of Knowledge, eds. Duveen, Gerard and Barbara Lloyd, 144–163. Cambridge: CUP.

  98. Bhargava, Rajeev. 1992. Individualism in Social Science. Forms and Limits of a Methodology. Oxford: Claredon Press.

  99. Battaile, M., Blin, J.F., Mias, C., and Piaser, A. 1997. Représentations sociales, représentations professionnelles, système des activités professionnelles. In L’Année de la recherche en sciences de l’education, 57–89. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

  100. Ratinaud, P., and M. Lac. 2011. Understanding Professionalization as a Representational Process. In Education, Professionalization and Social Representations, ed. M. Chaib, et al., 55–67. London: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203837207.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  101. Foucault, M. 1970, 1981. The Order of Discourse. Inaugural Lecture at the College de France, given 2 Dec 1970. In Untying the Text: A Post-Structuralist Reader, ed. R Young, pages. Routledge & Kegan Paul.

  102. Moloney, G., et al. 2005. Social Representations and Themata: The Construction and Functioning of Social Knowledge about Donation and Transplantation. British Journal of Social Psychology 44 (3): 415–441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  103. Abric, J.C. 1993. Central System, Peripheral System: Their Functions and Roles in the Dynamics of Social Representations. Papers on Social Representations 2 (2): 75–78.

    Google Scholar 

  104. Rosch, G. 1978. Principles of Categorization. In Cognition and Categorization, eds. Rosch, E., Lloyd, B.B., 27–48. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  105. Rosch, E., and C. Mervis. 1975. Family Resemblances: Studies in the Internal Structure of Categories. Cognitive Psychology 7 (4): 573–605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  106. Hart, H.L.A. 1994. The Concept of Law, 2nd ed. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  107. Heck, P. 1914. Gesetzauslegung und Interessenjurisprudenz. Tübingen: J.C.B. Mohr.

    Google Scholar 

  108. Heck, P. 1932. Begriffsbildung und Interessenjurisprudenz. Tübinhen: J.C.B. Mohr.

    Google Scholar 

  109. Hlouch, L. 2011. Teorie a realita právní interpretace. Plzeň: Aleš Čeněk.

    Google Scholar 

  110. Melzer, F. 2011. Metodologie nalézání práva. Úvod do právní Argumentace. Praha: C. H. Beck.

    Google Scholar 

  111. Daneš, F. 1966. The Relation of Centre and Periphery as a Language Universal. In Travaux linguistiques de Prague 2: 9–21.

    Google Scholar 

  112. Skrebtsova, T. 2014. The Concepts Centre and Periphery in the History of Linguistics: From Field Theory to Modern Cognitivism. Respectus Philologicus 26 (31): 144 ff.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  113. Vachek, J. 1966. On the Integration of the Peripheral Elements into the System of Language. Travaux linguistiques de Prague 2: 23–37.

    Google Scholar 

  114. Staerklé, C., A. Clémence, and D. Spini. 2011. Social Representations: A Normative and Dynamic Intergroup Approach. Political Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2011.00839.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  115. Jovchelovitch, S. 2008. The Rehabilitation of Common Sense: Social Representations, Science and Cognitive Polyphasia. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 38 (4): 431–448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  116. Chaib, M., and J. Chaib. 2011. Teacher Student’s Social Representations of How Adults Learn. In C Education, Professionalization and Social Representations, ed. M. Chaib, et al., 123–133. London: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203837207.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  117. Borghi, A.M. 2022. Concepts for Which We Need Others More: The Case of Abstract Concepts. Current Directions in Psychological Science 31 (3): 238–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  118. Asmuth, J., and D. Gentner. 2017. Relational Categories are More Mutable Than Entity Categories. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 70 (10): 2007–2025. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1219752.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  119. Wang, X., and Y. Bi. 2021. Idiosyncratic Tower of Babel: Individual Differences in Word-Meaning Representation Increase as Word Abstractness Increases. Psychological Science 32 (10): 1617–1635.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  120. Villani, C., L. Lugli, M.T. Liuzza, and A.M. Borghi. 2019. Varieties of Abstract Concepts and Their Multiple Dimensions. Language and Cognition 11 (3): 403–430. https://doi.org/10.1017/langcog.2019.232019.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  121. Roversi, C. 2021. Cognitive Science and the Nature of Law. In Law and Mind A Survey of Law and the Cognitive Sciences, ed. J. Hage, B. Brożek, and N. Vincent, 99–137. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  122. Farr, R.M. 1993. Common Sense, Science and Social Representations. Public Understanding of Science 2 (3): 189–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  123. Margolis, E., and S. Laurence. 1999. Concepts. Core Readings. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  124. Krcmar, M., Haberkorn, K. 2020. Mental Representations. In International Encyclopaedia of Media Psychology. John Wiley & Sons. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119011071.iemp0191

  125. Friedman, L.S. 1975. The Legal System: A Social Science Perspective. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  126. Fekete, B. 2018. Inconsistencies in the Use of Legal Culture in Comparative Legal Studies. Maastricht Journal of European and Comparative Law 25 (5): 551–564. https://doi.org/10.1177/1023263X18796978.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  127. White, James Boyd. 2008. Establishing Relations Between Law and Other Forms of Thought and Language. Erasmus Law Review 1 (3): 3–22.

    Google Scholar 

  128. Weyr, F. 1936. Teorie práva. Praha: Orbis.

    Google Scholar 

  129. Weyr, F. 1920. Základy filosofie právní. Brno: A Píša.

    Google Scholar 

  130. Weyr, F. 1930. Teorie a Praxe. Časopis pro právní a státní vědu 13: 253–265.

    Google Scholar 

  131. Kühn, Z. 2011. The Judiciary in Central and Eastern Europe: Mechanical Jurisprudence in Transformation? Leiden, Boston: Martin Nijhoff Publishers.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

The paper titled ‘Legal concepts as social representations’ has been created as a part of the project Využití metod sociální reprezentace v analýze právních konceptů [Method of social representation in the analysis of legal concepts], funded by Czech Science Foundation, Project Identification No GA20-10171S. The present paper is a substantial expansion on the basic theory published in Czech in Smejkalová et al. [57] Veřejný pořádek, důstojnost soudce a judikatura: Tři studie využití přístupu sociálních reprezentací v analýze právních konceptů (Brno: MUNIPress).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Terezie Smejkalová.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Smejkalová, T. Legal Concepts as Social Representations. Int J Semiot Law 37, 165–188 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11196-023-10066-3

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11196-023-10066-3

Keywords

Navigation