Skip to main content
Log in

Affect, agency and responsibility: The act of killing in the age of cyborgs

  • Published:
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

I explore the role of affect (rages and panics) and pre-cognitive reflexes in enabling killing in infantry combat. I examine Vietnam-era infantry training, which constructed a practical agent of killing which operated at an emergent group level, using the trained reflexes of individual soldiers as its components. I show that individual soldiers sometimes retrospectively took guilt upon themselves (a responsibility that is traditionally reserved for acts of individual conscious intention) even though the practical agent was the group activating the non-subjective reflexes of the individual soldiers. To explain this phenomenon, I explore proto-empathetic identification, which produces psychological trauma at the sight of the blood and guts of the killed enemy, despite the common practice of dehumanization of the enemy. I also examine cutting-edge digital and video simulator training for urban warfare of the “shoot/no shoot” type, which produces a very quick decision upon recognition of key traits of the situation—an act that is close to reflexive, but a bit more cognitively sophisticated. The same proto-empathetic identification and individual guilt assumption is in play in this training regime, even as the use of real-time communication technology forms ever more distributed group cognition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. As the anonymous reviewer of the paper reminded us, the standard evolutionary explanation for the adoption of signaling rather than fighting among non-human animal conspecifics does not involve empathetic identification but rather an instinctually embedded cost-benefit analysis. For example, the risk of harm from a fight outweighs the benefits of mating, so that it is better to accept defeat and wait to find another opportunity later.

  2. Here we use computer metaphors about which we have to be very careful not to let them imply any stance on cognition as computation of discrete symbols.

  3. With thanks for research help to Jane Richardson, and for comments and questions to many attendees of the “Cognition: Embodied, Embedded, Enactive, Extended” conference organized by Shaun Gallagher at the University of Central Florida, October 20–24, 2007.

References

  • Archer, J. (1988). The behavioral biology of aggression. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arquilla, J., & Rondfeldt, D. (2000). Swarming and the Future of Conflict. Santa Monica, RAND.

  • Burke, C. (2004). Camp all-American, Hanoi Jane, and the high-and-tight: gender, folklore, and changing military culture. Boston: Beacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Casey, E. (2000). Remembering: A phenomenological study. Bloomington: Indiana Univ. Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Correll, J., Urland, G., & Ito, T. (2006). Event-related potentials and the decision to shoot: the role of threat perception and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 120–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes’ error. New York: Avon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens. New York: Harcourt.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. (1997). Good natured. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, J. (1999). Using networked simulation to assess problem solving by tactical teams. Computers in Human Behavior, 15, 375–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2005). How the body shapes the mind. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. (2001). The ‘Shared Manifold’ hypothesis: from mirror neurons to empathy. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(5–7), 33–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(9), 396–403 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. A. (1977). Drug effects on fear and frustration: Possible limbic site of action of minor tranquilizers. In L. L. Iversen, S. D. Iversen, & S. H. Snyder (Eds.), Handbook of psychopharmacology, volume 8: Drugs, transmitters, and behavior (pp. 433–529). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, P. (1997). What emotions really are. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossman, D. (1996). On killing. Boston: Little, Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hendriks-Jansen, H. (1996). Catching ourselves in the act. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoge, C., Castro, C., Messer, S., McGurk, D., Cotting, D., & Koffman, R. (2004). Combat duty in Iraq and Afghanistan, mental health problems, and barriers to care. New England Journal of Medicine, 351, 13–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hurley, S. (2004). Imitation, media violence, and free speech. Philosophical Studies, 117(1–2), 165–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kilner, P. (2000). Military leaders’ obligation to justify killing in war. Presentation to the Joint Services Conference on Professional Ethics, Washington DC, Jan 27–28, 2000. http://www.usafa.edu/JSCOPE00/Kilner00.html.

  • Kirkland, F. (1995). Postcombat reentry. In War psychiatry. Textbook of military medicine, part I. Washington DC: Office of the Surgeon General.

  • LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: Simon and Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lifton, R. (1973). Home from the war. New York: Simon and Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Macedonia, M. (2002). Games, Simulation, and the Military Education Dilemma. http://www.educause.edu/ir/library/pdf/ffpiu018.pdf

  • Maibom, H. (2007). The presence of others. Philosophical Studies, 132(2), 161–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, S. (1978). Men against fire. Norman OK: Oklahoma University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarter, M. (2005). Lights! Camera! Training! Military Training Technology 10.2. http://www.military-training-technology.com/article.cfm?DocID=949

  • McNeill, W. (1995). Keeping together in time. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niehoff, D. (1999). The biology of violence. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pierson, D. (1999). Natural killers—turning the tide of battle. Military Review (May–June 1999): 60–65.

  • Shay, J. (1994). Achilles in Vietnam. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science, 303, 1157–1162 (20 Feb 2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Speidel, M. (2002). Berserks: a history of Indo-European ‘Mad Warriors’. Journal of World History, 13(2), 253–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York: Basic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, E. (2001). Empathy and consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(5–7), 1–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • van der Kolk, B., & Greenberg, M. (1987). The psychobiology of the trauma response. In B. van der Kolk (Ed.), Psychological trauma. Washington DC: American Psychiatric.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, D. (2005). Subjectivity and selfhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John Protevi.

Additional information

This article would not have reached this stage without Roger Pippin (Communication, University of South Florida) who was co-author of the first draft. As the paper has developed, however, we have decided it would be best for Protevi to assume authorship of this article and for us to pursue our joint project in another publication.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Protevi, J. Affect, agency and responsibility: The act of killing in the age of cyborgs. Phenom Cogn Sci 7, 405–413 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-008-9097-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-008-9097-z

Keywords

Navigation