Skip to main content
Log in

The unconscious side of Facebook: Do online social network profiles leak cues to users’ implicit motive dispositions?

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this study we investigated the links between motive dispositions and online social network (OSN) profile content. We assessed the achievement, affiliation and power motives via self- and peer-report. In addition, we used a projective test and two novel, affect based measures (involving affect ratings and EMG recordings) to assess implicit motives in the three content domains. Two observers independently coded motive-specific OSN content. Results showed that self-reports were linked to OSN content for the power domain. Peer-reports and measures of implicit motives were positively linked to OSN content across motive domains. In most cases, measures of implicit motives were still linked to OSN content after adjusting for self- and peer-reports. These results indicate that OSN profiles may leak cues to users’ implicit motives, which neither users themselves nor their peers are aware of. Implications for motive theory, motive assessment, and targeted online advertising will be discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We computed a series of independent samples t-tests to explore whether the current subsample differed from the remaining participants in their motive dispositions. Out of the 15 tests (5 measures from each motive domain), only one was significant at the 0.05 level. That is, participants from the current subsample had reduced levels of the EMG-assessed power motive, t(205) = 2.15, p = 0.03. Given the large number of tests, however, it seems possible that this finding might have been a false positive result. In any case, the analyses did not indicate that there is a substantial overall difference between the current subsample and the remaining participants in terms of their motive dispositions. Similarly, there was no age difference between the current subsample and the remaining participants, t(207) = 0.99, p = 0.32. There was a significant difference, however, in terms of sex composition, χ2(1) = 5.15, p = 0.02. Among the current subsample, the proportion of women was larger (78%) than among the remaining participants (61%). Thus, women were over-represented in the subsample.

  2. The study also contained a self-developed IAT to assess motive dispositions. However, IAT scores did not consistently correlate with any other motive measures any other outcome variables in the larger data set, or OSN content. Because this points towards limited validity of the IAT, we refrained from including the IAT in our analyses.

  3. The study also contained the following cues that were coded, but not used as indicators of motive dispositions: number of pictures, number of pictures with person on it, number of received likes in reaction to a post, amount of posts, posts disclosing personal information, funny/cheerful posts, posts with appeal character, amount of likes, likes disclosing personal information, likes with appeal character, likes with funny/cheerful content, and likes concerning other persons.

  4. The positive correlation between EMG assessments of the affiliation motive and affiliative OSN content was also reported in the article by Dufner et al. (2015).

References

  • Atkinson, J. W., Bongort, K., & Price, L. H. (1977). Explorations using computer simulation to comprehend thematic apperceptive measurement of motivation. Motivation and Emotion, 1, 1–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Banse, R., Imhoff, R., Steffens, M., Schramm, N., Rösch, A., Roberts, M., & Stangier, U. (2013). Partner-AMP and well-being: Evidence for an implicit secure base script? Personal Relationships, 20, 140–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bergman, S. M., Fearrington, M. E., Davenport, S. W., & Bergman, J. Z. (2011). Millennials, narcissism, and social networking: What narcissists do on social networking sites and why. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 706–711.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bosson, J. K., Swann, W. B., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2000). Stalking the perfect measure of implicit self-esteem: The blind men and the elephant revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 631–643.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brunstein, J. C. (2008). Implicit and explicit motives. In J. Heckhausen & H. Heckhausen (Eds.), Motivation and action (3rd ed., pp. 227–246). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Grässman, R. (1998). Personal goals and emotional well-being: The moderating role of motive dispositions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 494–508.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Losch, M. E., & Kim, H. S. (1986). Electromyographic activity over facial muscle regions can differentiate the valence and intensity of affective reactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 260–268.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carreon, B. (2011). How to say you’re the boss with your clothes [Blog post]. Retrieved from https://www.forbes.com/sites/bluecarreon/2011/07/19/how-to-say-youre-the-boss-with-your-clothes/#26433ee12e3e.

  • De Houwer, J., Thomas, S., & Baeyens, F. (2001). Associative learning of likes and dislikes: A review of 25 years of research on human evaluative conditioning. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 853–869.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dufner, M., Arslan, R. C., Hagemeyer, B., Schönbrodt, F. D., & Denissen, J. J. A. (2015). Affective contingencies in the affiliative domain: Physiological assessment, associations with the affiliation motive, and prediction of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109, 662–676.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Facebook (2015). Retrieved from http://newsroom.fb.com/company-info/.

  • Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1013–1027.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human facial expression. San Diego, CA: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fridlund, A. J., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Guidelines for human electromyographic research. Psychophysiology, 23, 567–589.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, L. T., Sandy, C. J., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Manifestations of individual differences in physical and virtual environments. In T. Chamorro-Premuzic, von Stumm S., & A. Furnham (Eds.), Handbook of individual differences (pp. 773–800). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hagemeyer, B., Dufner, M., & Denissen, J. J. (2016). Double dissociation between implicit and explicit affiliative motives: A closer look at socializing behavior in dyadic interactions. Journal of Research in Personality, 65, 89–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H. (1963). Hoffnung und Furcht in der Leistungsmotivation [Hope and fear in achievement motivation]. Meisenheim am Glan: Anton Hain.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heser, K., Banse, R., & Imhoff, R. (2015). Affiliation or power: What motivates behavior on social networking sites? Swiss Journal of Psychology, 74, 37–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, D. N. (1967). Manual for the Personality Research Form. Goshen, IN: Research Psychologist Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Köllner, M. G., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2014). Meta-analytic evidence of low convergence between implicit and explicit measures of the needs for achievement, affiliation, and power. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 826.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kosinski, M., Stillwell, D., & Graepel, T. (2013). Private traits and attributes are predictable from digital records of human behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 5802–5805.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krause, S., Back, M. D., Egloff, B., & Schmukle, S. C. (2016). Predicting self-confident behavior with implicit and explicit self-esteem measures. European Journal of Personality, 30, 648–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krause, S., & Dufner, M. (2017). Predicting romantic relationship behaviors with explicit and implicit partner evaluations: An actor-partner interdependence analysis. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Larsen, J. T., Norris, C. J., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2003). Effects of positive and negative affect on electromyographic activity over zygomaticus major and corrugator supercilii. Psychophysiology, 40, 776–785.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McAdams, D. P., Jackson, R. J., & Kirshnit, C. (1984). Looking laughing and smiling in dyads as a function of intimacy motivation. Journal of Personality, 52, 261–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C. (1987). Human motivation. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C., Koestner, R., & Weinberger, J. (1989). How do self-attributed and implicit motives differ? Psychological Review, 96, 690–702.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pang, J. S., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2005). Assessing implicit motives in US college students: Effects of picture type and position, gender and ethnicity, and cross-cultural comparisons. Journal of Personality Assessment, 85, 280–294.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, M., Abraham, C., & Bond, R. (2012). Psychological correlates of university students’ academic performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 353–387.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rudolph, A., Schröder-Abé, M., Schütz, A., Gregg, A. P., & Sedikides, C. (2008). Through a glass, less darkly? Reassessing convergent and discriminant validity in measures of implicit self-esteem. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 24, 273–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schönbrodt, F. D., & Gerstenberg, F. X. (2012). An IRT analysis of motive questionnaires: The unified motive scales. Journal of Research in Personality, 6, 725–742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C. (2001). An information processing account of implicit motive arousal. In M. L. Maehr & P. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 12, pp. 1–41). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C. (2008). Implicit motives. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 603–633). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C., & Pang, J. S. (2007). Measuring implicit motives. In R. W. Robins, R. C. Fraley & R. F. Krueger (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in personality psychology (pp. 323–344). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scinta, A., & Gable, S. L. (2007). Automatic and self-reported attitudes in romantic relationships. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1008–1022

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sedikides, C., Hoorens, V., & Dufner, M. (2015). Self-enhancing self-presentation: Interpersonal, relational, and organizational implications. In F. Guay, D. M. McInerney, R. Craven & H. W. Marsh (Eds.), Selfconcept, motivation and identity: Underpinning success with research and practice. International advances in self research (Vol. 5, pp. 29–55). Charlotte, NC: Information Age.

  • Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 220–247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stumpf, H., Angleitner, A., Wieck, T., Jackson, D. N., & Beloch-Till, H. (1985). Deutsche Personality Research Form (PRF): Handanweisung [German personality research form: Manual]. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tassinary, L. G., Hess, U., & Carcoba, L. M. (2012). Peripheral physiological measures of psychological constructs. In H. Cooper (Ed.), APA handbook of research methods in psychology, Vol. 1: Foundations, planning, measures, and psychometrics (pp. 461–488). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Vazire, S., & Carlson, E. N. (2011). Others sometimes know us better than we know ourselves. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 104–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107, 101–126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Winter, D. G. (1994). Manual for scoring motive imagery in running text (4th ed.). Ann Arbor: Department of Psychology, University of Michigan (unpublished manuscript).

  • Youyou, W., Kosinski, M., & Stillwell, D. (2015). Computer-based personality judgments are more accurate than those made by humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112, 1036–1040.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by Grant DE 1662/2-1 from the German Research Foundation (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft) allocated to Jaap J. A. Denissen. We thank Birk Hagemeyer for helpful comments of an earlier version of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael Dufner.

Appendix

Appendix

See Table 5.

Table 5 Correlations between motive measure and OSN content in the respective domains separately for the two waves (w1/w2)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Dufner, M., Arslan, R.C. & Denissen, J.J.A. The unconscious side of Facebook: Do online social network profiles leak cues to users’ implicit motive dispositions?. Motiv Emot 42, 79–89 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-017-9663-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-017-9663-1

Keywords

Navigation