Skip to main content
Log in

How expected evaluation influences creativity: Regulatory focus as moderator

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two studies investigated the effect of expected evaluation and regulatory focus on individuals’ creative performance. In both studies, first, the type of evaluation (informational versus controlling) was manipulated, and then regulatory focus was measured as an individual difference (in Study 1) or induced as a state using a pencil-and-paper maze task (in Study 2). Results provided evidence that participants who expect an informational evaluation were more likely to adopt an eager strategy; whereas participants who expected a controlling evaluation were more likely to adopt a vigilant strategy. Furthermore, participants in promotion-informational and prevention-controlling groups (regulatory fit conditions) performed more creatively than those in promotion-controlling and prevention-informational groups (regulatory non-fit conditions). In sum, the present findings contribute to a better understanding of how external evaluations and basic motivational orientations influence creative performance.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Amabile, T. M. (1979). Effects of external evaluation on artistic creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(2), 221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amabile, T. M. (1983). The social psychology of creativity. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Amabile, T. M., & Gryskiewicz, S. (1987). Creativity in the R & D laboratory (Tech. Rep. No. 30). Greensboro, NC: Center for Creative Leadership.

    Google Scholar 

  • Avnet, T., & Higgins, E. T. (2003). Locomotion, assessment, and regulatory fit: Value transfer from “how” to “what”. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(5), 525–530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K., & Nijstad, B. A. (2011). When prevention promotes creativity: The role of mood, regulatory focus, and regulatory closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(5), 794–809.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bartis, S., Szymanski, K., & Harkins, S. G. (1988). Evaluation and performance a two-edged knife. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 14(2), 242–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cesario, J., & Higgins, E. T. (2008). Making message recipients “feel right” how nonverbal cues can increase persuasion. Psychological Science, 19(5), 415–420.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cesario, J., Higgins, E. T., & Scholer, A. A. (2008). Regulatory fit and persuasion: Basic principles and remaining questions. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(1), 444–463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chernev, A. (2004). Goal-attribute compatibility in consumer choice. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(1), 141–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, J. F., & Richter, A. W. (2006). Probing three-way interactions in moderated multiple regression: Development and application of a slope difference test. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 917–926.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1980). The empirical exploration of intrinsic motivational processes. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 39–80). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ding, X. Q., Tang, Y. Y., Tang, R. X., & Posner, M. I. (2014). Improving creativity performance by short-term meditation. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 10(9), 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Förster, J., Higgins, E. T., & Idson, L. C. (1998). Approach and avoidance strength during goal attainment: Regulatory focus and the” goal looms larger” effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(5), 1115.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, R. S., & Förster, J. (2001). The effects of promotion and prevention cues on creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(6), 1001–1013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gino, F., & Margolis, J. D. (2011). Bringing ethics into focus: How regulatory focus and risk preferences influence (un) ethical behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 115(2), 145–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamstra, M. R., Van Yperen, N. W., Wisse, B., & Sassenberg, K. (2013). Like or dislike: Intrapersonal regulatory fit affects the intensity of interpersonal evaluation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(4), 726–731.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haws, K., Dholakia, U., & Bearden, W. O. (2010). An assessment of chronic regulatory focus measures. Journal of Marketing Research, 47, 967–982.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herman, A., & Reiter-Palmon, R. (2011). The effect of regulatory focus on idea generation and idea evaluation. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 5(1), 13–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herman, A. E. (2008). The influence of regulatory focus, expected evaluation, and goal orientation on cognitive processes related to creative problem solving. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, University of Nebraska.

  • Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52, 1280–1300.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and prevention: Regulatory focus as a motivational principle. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 1–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T. (2000). Making a good decision: Value from fit. American Psychologist, 55(11), 1217–1230.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T. (2005). Value from regulatory fit. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(4), 209–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T., Friedman, R. S., Harlow, R. E., Idson, L. C., Ayduk, O. N., & Taylor, A. (2001). Achievement orientations from subjective histories of success: Promotion pride versus prevention pride. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 3–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T., Idson, L. C., Freitas, A. L., Spiegel, S., & Molden, D. C. (2003). Transfer of value from fit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(6), 1140–1153.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Idson, L. C., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E. T. (2004). Imagining how you’d feel: The role of motivational experiences from regulatory fit. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(7), 926–937.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, J., & Bless, H. (2006). Regulatory fit and cognitive performance: The interactive effect of chronic and situationally induced self-regulatory mechanisms on test performance. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36(3), 393–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, K. H., Lee, H. E., Chae, K.-B., Anderson, L., & Laurence, C. (2011). Creativity and Confucianism among American and Korean educators. Creativity Research Journal, 23(4), 357–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, K. H., & VanTassel-Baska, J. (2010). The relationship between creativity and behavior problems among underachieving elementary and high school students. Creativity Research Journal, 22(2), 185–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lam, T. W. H., & Chiu, C. Y. (2002). The motivational function of regulatory focus in creativity. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 36(2), 138–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69, 220–232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Otto, A. R., Markman, A. B., Gureckis, T. M., & Love, B. C. (2010). Regulatory fit and systematic exploration in a dynamic decision-making environment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(3), 797.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Qu, X. J., & Shi, J. N. (2005). Evaluation and reward effect on verbal creativity of field dependent-independent children. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 6, 749–753.

    Google Scholar 

  • Runco, M. A., Illies, J. J., & Eisenman, R. (2005). Creativity, originality, and appropriateness: What do explicit instructions tell us about their relationships? Journal of Creative Behavior, 39, 137–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scibinetti, P., Tocci, N., & Pesce, C. (2011). Motor creativity and creative thinking in children: The diverging role of inhibition. Creativity Research Journal, 23(3), 262–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shah, J., Higgins, T., & Friedman, R. S. (1998). Performance incentives and means: How regulatory focus influences goal attainment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 285–293.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shalley, C. E. (1995). Effects of coaction, expected evaluation, and goal setting on creativity and productivity. Academy of Management Journal, 38(2), 483–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shalley, C. E., & Oldham, G. R. (1985). Effects of goal difficulty and expected external evaluation on intrinsic motivation: A laboratory study. Academy of Management Journal, 28(3), 628–640.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shalley, C. E., & Perry-Smith, J. E. (2001). Effects of social-psychological factors on creative performance: The role of informational and controlling expected evaluation and modeling experience. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 84(1), 1–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smeltz, W., & Cross, B. (1984). Toward a profile of the creative R & D professional. IEEE Transactions on Engineering Management, EM-31, 22–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spiegel, S., Grant-Pillow, H., & Higgins, E. T. (2004). How regulatory fit enhances motivational strength during goal pursuit. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34(1), 39–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torrance, E. P. (1966). The Torrance tests of creative thinking-norms-technical manual research edition-verbal tests, forms A and B-figural tests, forms A and B. Princeton, NJ: Personnel Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torrance, E. P. (1974). Torrance tests of creative thinking: Norms and technical manual. Bensenville, IL: Scholostic Testing Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, S. M., Vendramini, C. M. M., & Oakland, T. (2012). Thinking and creative styles: A validity study. Creativity Research Journal, 24(2–3), 235–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Werth, L., & Förster, J. (2007). The effects of regulatory focus on braking speed 1. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 37(12), 2764–2787.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wu, J. J., Gao, C. F., Wang, J. Y., & Ding, S. S. (1981). Torrance tests of creative thinking: Figural form A (in Chinese). Taibei: Yuan Liu Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yuan, F., & Zhou, J. (2008). Differential effects of expected external evaluation on different parts of the creative idea production process and on final product creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 20(4), 391–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ling Wang.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Appendix

Appendix

Instruction of the informational evaluation

Later, experts in human resources will carefully review your ideas in this task. We need this review as part of the study. We will provide you with a copy so that you can learn from this study, since we have their evaluations. They may tell you what they liked about your responses and/or suggest alternative approaches or improvements on what you did. I’m sure each of you will find this information useful because creative problem solving is highly valued and will help you in the real world. Anyone can solve problems by coming up with typical solutions, the same old thing everyone else would suggest, but the employee who is creative and offers unique ideas stands out. So, the feedback from the evaluators will help you learn something that will be useful beyond this study and beyond the school setting. Now, remember we are interested in you trying to be creative. I will be asking you later for an address where I can mail the reviews to you.

Instruction of the controlling evaluation

Now, you have a creativity goal, and we expect you to be creative. This is vitally important to us, and we expect you to generate creative ideas for this study. In fact, your data is needed to complete this study. Now you are going to be judged on how creative you are by experts in human resources, so they are knowledgeable and tough. These experts will critically evaluate your performance in this task by analyzing every thought you have in the memo and judging if it is creative or not. We will send you your score so that you know if you performed as you should have. You’ll be sent your score and told how your score compared to what we wanted. Remember, you should be creative. I will be asking you later for an address where I can mail your score to you.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wang, J., Wang, L., Liu, RD. et al. How expected evaluation influences creativity: Regulatory focus as moderator. Motiv Emot 41, 147–157 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-016-9598-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-016-9598-y

Keywords

Navigation