Skip to main content
Log in

Can’t get you out of my mind: empathy, distress, and recurring thoughts about a person in need

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Research suggests that empathic concern and distress give rise to different patterns of helping behavior. It has been proposed that this difference is caused by the effects of these emotions on recurrent thoughts about the person in need. However, no research has directly investigated this potential explanation. To remedy this, we tested the hypotheses that distress, but not empathic concern, is associated with both anticipated recurring thoughts (Study 1) and experienced recurring thoughts (Study 2) about a victim. We also tested the hypothesis that distress is associated with thoughts about the victim, whereas empathic concern is associated with thoughts about the victim’s situation (Study 3), which is potentially a consequence of the motives associated with each emotion. Lastly, we assessed the causal relations between distress, empathic concern, and recurrent thoughts (Study 4). Overall, results demonstrate a distinctive, and important, pattern of associations among empathic concern, distress, and different forms of recurrent thoughts about the emotion-eliciting stimulus.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In order to ensure that empathy and personal distress were two different factors, we conducted a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) with the samples from the four studies (n = 233). AMOS software was used to conduct the CFA using a Weighted Least Squares Minimum Value (WLSMV) procedure, as this seems to be the most appropriate for polychoric correlations (Muthén and Muthén 1998-2012). We determined the fit of the model based on the goodness-of-fit indices (GOF). The GOF indices used in this study were: (a) The root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA); (b) the comparative fit index (CFI); and (c) the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI). RMSEA is considered acceptable at values lower than 0.06 (Hu and Bentler 1999). CFI and TLI are considered to give evidence of acceptable fit at values over a .90 threshold (Bentler and Bonett 1980), and excellent fit at .95 (Hu and Bentler 1999). Results showed an acceptable fit, with an RMSEA of 0.079 (p (RMSEA < 0.06) > .05), CFI = .967, and TLI = .943).

    figure a

    We also tested the fit of a single factor model. This model did not have an acceptable fit (RMSEA = 0.169; CFI = .836; and TLI = .779), and was significantly worse than the 2-factor solution (Δχ2 = 90.392, p value < .00005). Thus, empathic concern and distress, as measured by the ERQ in the present research, are two separate constructs.

  2. There were no gender differences on key dependent variables in any of the four studies (Study 1, ts < .14, ps > .88; Study 2, ts < .49, ps > .62; Study 3, ts < 1.22, ps > .23; Study 4, ts < 1.32, ps > .19), so gender is not included in the reported analyses.

  3. This pattern of results for empathic concern and distress is common for victim scenarios in which the suffering is not particularly graphic or shocking (for a review, see Batson 2011). The same pattern was found in Study 2 [empathic concern M = 3.99, SD = 1.27; distress M = 3.48, SD = 1.42, t (59) = 3.82, p < .001] and in Study 3 [empathic concern M = 4.30, SD = 1.31; distress M = 3.42, SD = 1.48, t (43) = 5.45, p < .001]. In need situations involving graphic or disturbing content (e.g., blood, extreme pain), the distress tends to be stronger than empathic concern (see Hoffman, 2001, for a discussion of empathic over-arousal).

References

  • Batson, C. D. (1991). The altruism question: Toward a social-psychological answer. Hillsdale, NJ: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Batson, C. D. (2011). Altruism in humans. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Batson, C. D., & Ahmad, N. (2009). Using empathy to improve intergroup attitudes and relations. Social Issues and Policy Review, 3, 141–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Batson, C. D., Bolen, M. H., Cross, J. A., & Neuringer-Benefiel, H. (1986). Where is the altruism in the altruistic personality? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 212–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Batson, C. D., & Coke, J. S. (1981). Empathy: A source of altruistic motivation for helping? In J. P. Rushton & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Altruism and helping behavior: Social, personality, and developmental perspectives (pp. 167–187). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Batson, C. D., Cowles, C., & Coke, J. S. (1979). Empathic mediation of the response to a lady in distress: Egoistic or altruistic? Unpublished manuscript, University of Kansas.

  • Batson, C. D., Dyke, J. L., Brandt, J. R., Batson, J. G., Powell, A. L., McCaster, M. R., et al. (1988). Five studies testing two new egoistic alternatives to the empathy-altruism hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 52–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Batson, C. D., Eklund, J. H., Chermok, V. L., Hoyt, J. L., & Ortiz, B. G. (2007). An additional antecedent of empathic concern: Valuing the welfare of the person in need. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 65–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Batson, C. D., O’Quin, K., Fultz, J., Vanderplas, M., & Isen, A. (1983). Self-reported distress and empathy and egoistic versus altruistic motivation for helping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 706–718.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bentler, P. M., & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 588–606.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheetham, M., Pedroni, A. F., Antley, A., Slater, M., & Jancke, L. (2009). Virtual milgram: Emphathic concern or personal distress? Evidence from functional MRI and dispositional measures. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 3, 29.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cialdini, R. B., & Kenrick, D. T. (1976). Altruism as hedonism: a social development perspective on the relationship of negative mood state and helping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 907–914.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coke, J. S. (1980). Empathic mediation of helping: Egoistic or altruistic? Doctoral dissertation, University of Kansas.

  • Coke, J. S., Batson, C. D., & McDavis, K. (1978). Empathic mediation of helping: A two-stage model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 752–766.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R., Grabowski, T. J., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Ponto, L. L., Parvizi, J., et al. (2000). Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 1049–1056.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M. (2006). Empathy: A social-psychological approach. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M. H., Conklin, L., Smith, A., & Luce, C. (1996). Effect of perspective taking on the cognitive representation of persons: A merging of self and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 713–726.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dovidio, J. F., Piliavin, J. A., Schroeder, D. A., & Penner, L. A. (2006). The social psychology of prosocial behavior. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman-Barrett, L., & Russell, J. A. (1998). Independence and bipolarity in the structure of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 967–984.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fultz, J. N. (1982). Influence of potential for self-reward on egoistically and altruistically motivated helping. Unpublished Master’s thesis, University of Kansas.

  • Gemar, M. C., Kapur, S., Segal, Z. V., Brown, G. M., & Houle, S. (1996). Effects of self-generated sad mood on regional cerebral activity: A PET study in normal subjects. Depression, 4, 81–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, M. L. (1981). Is altruism part of human nature? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 121–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, M. L. (1991). Is empathy altruistic? Psychological Inquiry, 2, 131–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, M. L. (2001). A comprehensive theory of prosocial moral development. In D. Stipek & A. Bohart (Eds.), Constructive and destructive behavior (pp. 61–86). Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hornstein, H. A. (1991). Empathic distress and altruism: Still inseparable. Psychological Inquiry, 2, 133–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ingram, R. E., & Smith, T. W. (1984). Depression and internal versus external focus of attention. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8, 139–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joireman, J. A., Parrott, L. P., & Hammersla, J. (2002). Empathy and the self-absorption paradox: Support for the distinction between self-rumination and self-reflection. Self and Identity, 1, 53–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamm, C., Batson, C. D., & Decety, J. (2007). The neural substrate of Human empathy: Effects of perspective taking and cognitive appraisal. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 42–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Latane, B., & Darley, J. (1969). Bystander “Apathy.”. American Scientist, 57, 244–268.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • López-Pérez, B., Carrera, P., Ambrona, T., & Oceja, L. (2014). Testing the qualitative differences between empathy and personal distress: Measuring core affect and self-orientation. Social Science Journal, 51, 676–680.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maner, J. K., Luce, C. L., Neuberg, S. L., Cialdini, R. B., Brown, S., & Sagarin, B. J. (2002). The effects of perspective taking on motivations for helping: Still no evidence for altruism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1601–1610.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998-2012). Mplus user’s guide (7th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

  • Oceja, L. V., Ambrona, T., López-Pérez, B., Salgado, S., & Villegas, M. (2010a). When the victim is one among others: Empathy, awareness of others, and motivational ambivalence. Motivation and Emotion, 34, 110–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oceja, L., Heerdink, T., Stocks, E., Ambrona, T., López-Pérez, B., & Salgado, S. (2014). Empathy, awareness of others, and action: How feeling empathy for one-among-others motivates to help the others. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 36, 111–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oceja, L., Stocks, E., & Lishner, D. (2010b). Congruence between the target in need and the recipient of aid: The one-among-others effect. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 40, 2814–2828.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piliavin, J. A., Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., & Clark, R. D., III. (1981). Emergency intervention. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Regan, D., & Totten, J. (1975). Empathy and attribution: Turning observers into actors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 850–856.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A., Weiss, A., & Mendhelson, G. A. (1989). Affect grid: A single-item scale of pleasure and arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 493–502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salovey, P. (1992). Mood-induced self-focused attention. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 699–707.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science, 303, 1157–1162.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stocks, E. L., Lishner, D. A., & Decker, S. K. (2009). Altruism or psychological escape: Why does empathy promote prosocial behavior? European Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 649–655.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toi, M., & Batson, C. D. (1982). More evidence that empathy is a source of altruistic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 281–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trapnell, P. D., & Campbell, J. D. (1999). Private self-consciousness and the five-factor model of personality: Distinguishing rumination from reflection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(2), 284–304.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This project was funded by the grant PSI2014-53321-P, in which Luis Oceja is co-investigator.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Eric L. Stocks.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

Eric Stocks declares that he has no conflict of interest. Belen Lopez-Perez declares that she has no conflict of interest. Luis Oceja declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Stocks, E.L., López-Pérez, B. & Oceja, L.V. Can’t get you out of my mind: empathy, distress, and recurring thoughts about a person in need. Motiv Emot 41, 84–95 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-016-9587-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-016-9587-1

Keywords

Navigation