Skip to main content
Log in

The diagnosticity of color for emotional objects

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Object classification can be facilitated if simple diagnostic features can be used to determine class membership. Previous studies have found that simple shapes may be diagnostic for emotional content and automatically alter the allocation of visual attention. In the present study, we analyzed whether color is diagnostic of emotional content and tested whether emotionally diagnostic hues alter the allocation of visual attention. Reddish-yellow hues are more common in (i.e., diagnostic of) emotional images, particularly images with positive emotional content. An exogenous cueing paradigm was employed to test whether these diagnostic hues orient attention differently from other hues due to the emotional diagnosticity. In two experiments, we found that participants allocated attention differently to diagnostic hues than to non-diagnostic hues, in a pattern indicating a broadening of spatial attention when cued with diagnostic hues. Moreover, the attentional broadening effect was predicted by self-reported measures of affective style, linking the behavioral effect to emotional processes. These results confirm the existence and use of diagnostic features for the rapid detection of emotional content.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. However, one should note that this representation is not identical to that in the thalamus because it is based on a transformation of RGB image coordinates rather than the relative activation-level of photoreceptors with long, medium or short wavelength sensitivities. Using this more biologically accurate model requires physical measurements from the display used during original IAPS emotional rating task, which is not possible.

  2. Two-dimensional color histograms were constructed using the H (hue) and S (saturation) dimensions, and the emotional versus non-emotional effect was significant for six highly-saturated reddish hues (peak effect was t(358) = 5.10).

References

  • Aronoff, J. (2006). How we recognize angry and happy emotion in people, places and things. Cross-cultural Research, 40(1), 83–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aronoff, J., Barclay, A. M., & Stevenson, L. A. (1988). The recognition of threatening facial stimuli. Journal or Personality and Social Psychology, 54(4), 647–655.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aronoff, J., Woike, B. A., & Hyman, L. M. (1992). Which are the stimuli in facial displays of anger and happiness? Configurational bases of emotion recognition. Journal or Personality and Social Psychology, 62(6), 1050–1066.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Attrill, M. J. (2008). Red shirt colour is associated with long-term team success in English football. Journal of Sports Sciences, 26, 577–582.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bar, M., & Neta, M. (2007). Visual elements of subjective preference modulate amygdala activation. Neuropsychologia, 45.

  • Berlin, B., & Kay, P. (1969). Basic color terms. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., Millar, N., & White, J. (1995). Selective processing of negative information: Effects of clinical anxiety, concurrent depression, and awareness. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 532–536.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Changizi, M. A., Zhang, Q., & Shimojo, S. (2006). Bare skin, blood and the evolution of primate colour vision. Biology Letters, 2(2), 217–221.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Codispoti, M., Ferrari, V., & Bradley, M. M. (2007). Repetition and event-related potentials: Distinguishing early and late processes in affective picture perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(4), 577–586.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. J. (1993). Cerebral asymmetry and emotion: Conceptual and methodological conundrums. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 115–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delplanque, S., N’diaye, K., Scherer, K., & Grandjean, D. (2007). Spatial frequencies or emotional effects? A systematic measure of spatial frequencies for IAPS pictures by a discrete wavelet analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 165(1), 144–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J., & Maier, M. A. (2007). Color and psychological functioning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(5), 250–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J., Maier, M. A., Binser, M. J., Friedman, R., & Pekrun, R. (2009). The effect of red on avoidance behavior in achievement contexts. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 365–375.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J., Maier, M. A., Moller, A. C., Friedman, R., & Meinhardt, J. (2007). Color and psychological functioning: The effect of red on performance attainment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136(1), 165–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 359(1449), 1367–1377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2008). Approach-motivated positive affect reduces breadth of attention. Psychological Science, 19, 476–482.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Genovese, C. R., Lazar, N. A., & Nichols, T. (2002). Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate. Neuroimage, 15(4), 870–878.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goffaux, V., Jacques, C., Mouraux, A., Oliva, A., Schyns, P. G., & Rossion, B. (2005). Diagnostic colours contribute to the early stages of scene categorization: Behavioural and neurophysiological evidence. Visual Cognition, 12(6), 878–892.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, H. B., & Knapp, R. H. (1959). Time judgment, aesthetic preference, and need for achievement. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58(1), 140–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hagemann, N., Strauss, B., & Leiβing, J. (2008). When the referee sees red…. Psychological Science, 19(8), 769–771.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haruno, M., Kuroda, T., Doya, K., Toyama, K., Kimura, M., Samejima, K., et al. (2004). A neural correlate of reward-based behavioral learning in caudate nucleus: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of stochastic decision task. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 1660–1665.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heubeck, B. G., Wilkinson, R. B., & Cologon, J. (1998). A second look at Carver and White’s (1994) BIS/BAS scales. Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 785–800.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, R. A., & Barton, R. A. (2005). Red enhances human performance in contests. Nature, 435(7040), 293.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, J. S., & Olshausen, B. A. (2003). Timecourse of neural signatures of object recognition. Journal of Vision, 3(7), 499–512.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Junghofer, M., Bradley, M. M., Elbert, T. R., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Fleeting images: A new look at early emotion discrimination. Psychophysiology, 38(02), 175–178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knapp, R. H. (1962). Attitudes toward time and aesthetic choice. The Journal of Social Psychology, 56, 79–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H. W., Crombez, G., Van Damme, S., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2004). Does imminent threat capture and hold attention. Emotion, 4(3), 312–317.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lake, D. A., & Bryden, M. P. (1976). Handedness and sex differences in hemispheric asymmetry. Brain and Language, 3(2), 266–282.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1995). International affective picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. Gainesville, FL: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larson, C. L., Aronoff, J., Sarinopoulos, I. C., & Zhu, D. C. (2009). Recognizing threat: A simple geometric shape activates neural circuitry for threat detection. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, (Early Access), pp 1–13.

  • Larson, C. L., Aronoff, J., & Stearns, J. J. (2007). The shape of threat: Simple geometric forms evoke rapid and sustained capture of attention. Emotion, 7(3), 526–534.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leone, L. (2009). Testing conceptual distinctions among Carver and White’s (1994) BAS scales: A comment and extension on Smillie, Jackon and Dalgleish (2006). Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 54–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leone, L., Perugini, M., Bagozzi, R. P., Pierro, A., & Mannetia, L. (2001). Construct validity and generalizability of the Carver-White behavioral inhibition system/behavioral activation system scales. European Journal of Personality, 15, 373–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lichtenfeld, S., Maier, M. A., Elliot, A. J., & Pekrun, R. (2009). The semantic red effect: Processing the word red undermines intellectual performance. Journal of Social Psychology, 45, 1273–1276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liddell, B. J., Brown, K. J., Kemp, A. H., Barton, M. J., Das, P., Peduto, A., et al. (2005). A direct brainstem–amygdala–cortical ‘alarm’ system for subliminal signals of fear. Neuroimage, 24(1), 235–243.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Little, A. C., & Hill, R. A. (2007). Attribution to red suggests special role in dominance signaling. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 5(1), 161–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lotto, R. B., & Purves, D. (2004). Perceiving color. Review of Progress in Coloration and Related Topics, 34, 12–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maxwell, J. S., Shackman, A. J., & Davidson, R. J. (2005). Unattended facial expressions asymmetrically bias the concurrent processing of nonemotional information. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(9), 1386–1395. doi:10.1162/0898929054985437.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milinski, M., & Bakker, T. C. M. (1990). Female sticklebacks use male coloration in mate choice and hence avoid parasitized males. Nature, 344(6264), 330–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., & Williams, R. (1995). Attentional bias in anxiety and depression: The role of awareness. The British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 34(Pt 1), 17–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moller, A. C., Elliot, A. J., & Maier, M. A. (2009). Basic hue-meaning associations. Emotion, 9(6), 898–902.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, F. C., Sahakian, B. J., Rubinsztein, J. S., Michael, A., Rogers, R. D., Robbins, T. W., et al. (1999). Emotional bias and inhibitory control processes in mania and depression. Psychological Medicine, 29(6), 1307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nestor, A., & Tarr, M. J. (2008). Gender recognition of human faces using color. Psychological Science, 19(12), 1242–1246.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nomura, E. M., & Reber, P. J. (2008). A review of medial temporal lobe and caudate contributions to visual category learning. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 279–291.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 130(3), 466–478.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oliva, A., & Schyns, P. G. (2000). Colored diagnostic blobs mediate scene recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 176–210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pasley, B. N., Mayes, L. C., & Schultz, R. T. (2004). Subcortical discrimination of unperceived objects during binocular rivalry. Neuron, 42(1), 163–172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Reviews in Neuroscience, 13(1), 25–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pryke, S. R., & Griffith, S. C. (2006). Red dominates black: Agonistic signaling among head morphs in the colour polymorphic gouldian finch. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 273(1589), 949–957.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R. (1993). Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 510–532.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schyns, P. G. (1998). Diagnostic recognition: Task constraints, object information, and their interactions. Cognition, 67(1–2), 147–179.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Segerstrom, S. C. (2001). Optimism and attentional bias for negative and positive stimuli. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(10), 1334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Setchell, J. M., & Wickings, E. (2005). Dominance, status signals and coloration in male mandrills (mandrillus sphinx). Ethology, 111(1), 25–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smillie, L. D., Jackson, C. J., & Dalgleish, L. I. (2006). Conceptual distinctions among Carver and White’s (1994) scales: A reward-reactivity versus trait impulsivity perspective. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 1039–1050.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Surridge, A. K., Osorio, D., & Mundy, N. I. (2003). Evolution and selection of trichromatic vision in primates. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 18(4), 198–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tanaka, J., Weiskopf, D., & Williams, P. (2001). The role of color in high-level vision. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(5), 211–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). Distinct spatial frequency sensitivities for processing faces and emotional expressions. Nature Neuroscience, 6(6), 624–631.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wadlinger, H. A., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2006). Positive mood broadens visual attention to positive stimuli. Motivation and Emotion, 30, 89–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waitt, C. (2003). Evidence from rhesus macaques suggests that male coloration plays a role in female primate mate choice. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 270, 144–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walther, D. (2006). Appendix A. 2. Color opponencies for bottom-up attention. Interactions of visual attention and object recognition: Computational modeling, Algorithms, and psychophysics. Ph.D. Thesis, 98–99.

  • Watson, D. G., Blagrove, E., Evans, C., & Moore, L. (2012). Negative triangles: Simple geometric shapes convey emotional valence. Emotion, 12(1), 18–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whalen, P. J. (1998). Fear, vigilance, and ambiguity: Initial neuroimaging studies of the human amygdala. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7(6), 177–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whalen, P. J., Kagan, J., Cook, R. G., Davis, F. C., Kim, H., Polis, S., et al. (2004). Human amygdala responsivity to masked fearful eye whites. Science, 306(5704), 2061.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wurm, L. H., Legge, G. E., Isenberg, L. M., & Luebker, A. (1993). Color improves object recognition in normal and low vision. Journal of experimental psychology, 19, 899–911.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zald, D. H. (2003). The human amygdala and the emotional evaluation of sensory stimuli. Brain Research Reviews, 41, 88–123.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

BWM was supported by T32-HD007151.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Brenton W. McMenamin.

Appendix

Appendix

See Table 6.

Table 6 IAPS slides used in color analysis

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

McMenamin, B.W., Radue, J., Trask, J. et al. The diagnosticity of color for emotional objects. Motiv Emot 37, 609–622 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-012-9319-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-012-9319-0

Keywords

Navigation