Skip to main content
Log in

Language games and their types

  • Original Research
  • Published:
Linguistics and Philosophy Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

One of the success stories of formal semantics is explicating responsive moves like answers to questions. There is, however, a significant lacune concerning the characterization of initiating utterances, which are strongly tied to the conversational activity [language game (Wittgenstein), speech genre (Bakhtin)], or—our terminology—conversational type, one is engaged in. To date there has been no systematic proposal trying to account for the range of possible language games/speech genres/conversational types and their global structure. In particular, concerning the range of subject matter that can and needs to be discussed and by whom—ultimately a semantic analogue of Laplace’s demon. We suggest that the subject matter problem for conversational types is a central task for any semantic theory for conversation. This paper develops a theory of conversational types, which embedded in the theory of conversational interaction KoS, enables this problem to be tackled for a wide range of conversational types drawn from the British National Corpus classification of conversational domains. The theory we develop treats conversational types as first class, not metatheoretical entities, in contrast to explications of corresponding notions in game theoretic approaches. We demonstrate that this allows us to explicate the possibilities interlocutors have to refer to and seek clarification about the types of conversations they are engaged in.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. KoS is not an acronym, but an island in the Dodacanese archipelago. One could relate this name, nonetheless, to Conversation Oriented Semantics.

  2. The name originates in the British National Corpus genre taxonomy (Lee, 2002).

  3. For distributions of non-sentential utterances in different conversational types, see Wong and Ginzburg (2018).

  4. Whether all its dimensions should allow for such variation is an interesting question. Thus, whereas distinctness in subject matter seems a dimension which needs to be open, the number of participants, for instance, should not—conversational types can specify a fixed number of participants (2 or 4 players and one referee for a tennis match). At the same time, it does not seem to be the case that just any distinctness in number of participants will lead to individuating of a new type. For instance, lectures with six participants or lectures with seven participants are not ceteris paribus to be individuated as different types. But we will not resolve this important issue here.

  5. See, in particular, Chapter 11 of Osborne and Rubinstein (1994).

  6. Here grounding, following Clark (1996), refers to the process of establishing presuppositions that utterances are mutually understood.

  7. How to extend such rules to multi-party conversation is discussed in Ginzburg (2012, Section 8.1).

  8. Recall from the assertion protocol that asserting p introduces p? into QUD.

  9. In fact, for simplicity one could assume, as did Ginzburg (2012), that conversation-finally QUD needs to be empty. Given data about resumption of conversations, discussed by Ginzburg and Lücking (2020)—e.g., a question left hanging at the end of a conversation being answered when the interlocutors meet again— this assumption cannot be maintained.

  10. A useful feature of TTR (which distinguishes it from most versions of typed feature structures (Carpenter, 1992) is that

    figure w
  11. The basic idea of merge for record types is illustrated by the examples in (i), (ii).

    1. (i)
      figure z
    2. (ii)
      figure aa

    For a full definition which makes clear what the result is of merging any two arbitrary types, see Cooper (2012).

  12. In fact, this rule can be generalized for moves that do not introduce an issue, but we will not explore this here.

  13. There is an apparent technical question here: since conversational types on the definition we gave above make reference to conversational rules, do they make reference to this conversational rule as well? In such a case there is the risk of circularity, which although not necessarily technically malign (Barwise & Moss, 1996) seems uncalled for here. However, we make reference here only to the final state associated with the conversational type, not the entire conversational type. One could, nonetheless, claim that the predicate Projectable should make explicit reference to the conversational rules that will be used to reach from the current DGB to the final one. But this seems like an instance of the fallacy of misplaced concreteness, where we assume that the agent is using the conversational rules posited here.

  14. Assuming the arguments given in Ginzburg (2012) that parting presupposes that at least a minimal amount of interaction took place.

  15. This can be implemented in several ways, via a reformulation of the rule of Fact Update / QUD Downdate, which is where QNUD gets updated.

  16. This might be better viewed as a complex move.

  17. Source: https://hansard.parliament.uk/commons/2020-10-07/debates/772AEE6E-1634-4943-82C9-77211DE61F02/Engagements.

  18. Source: https://www.ourcommons.ca/documentviewer/en/house/latest/hansard.

  19. Source https://www.themuse.com/advice/interview-questions-and-answers.

  20. Should we assume that such interactions need to be covered by (a minor variant of) Bakery—strictly speaking Café), same QNUD, mildly different setting— conversational type? Or should we assume that such cases illustrate the dynamicity of conversational types, which after significant use potentially mutate into more abbreviated interactions (Garrod & Doherty, 1994; Healey & Mills, 2006)? We will not try to resolve this issue here. One could, for instance, analyze such cases in terms of the Brief Encounter type in conjunction with a description of the expected sequence of event types that do not involve dialogue.

  21. We are somewhat loosely using a ‘universal quantifier’ based condition here. This can be made precise in TTR in a number of different ways, most obviously in the standard explication of universals using dependent types, following Ranta (1994).

  22. Given the deep learning revolution in NLP, current approaches to question classification have changed, see e.g., Han et al. (2022) for discussion and exemplification.

  23. See discussion in Ginzburg (2012, Section 8.3).

  24. Cooper and Ginzburg (2015) suggest that for events with even a modicum of internal structure, one can enrich the type theory using the “String theory” developed by Tim Fernando [e.g., Fernando (2007)].

References

  • Abulmiti, A. (2020). Different games in dialogue. In S. Malamud, J. Pustejovsky, & J. Ginzburg (Eds.), Proceedings of SemDial 2020 (pp. 63–79). Brandeis University.

  • Allen, J., & Perrault, R. (1980). Analyzing intention in utterances. Artificial Intelligence, 15, 143–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allwood, J. (2000). An activity based approach of pragmatics. In H. Bunt (Ed.), Abduction, belief and context in dialogue: Studies in computational pragmatics (pp. 47–80). John Benjamins.

  • Asher, N., & Lascarides, A. (2003). Logics of conversation. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asher, N., Paul, S., & Venant, A. (2017). Message exchange games in strategic contexts. Journal of Philosophical Logic, 46(4), 355–404.

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, J. M., & Drew, P. (1979). Order in court: The organization of verbal behaviour in judicial settings. Humanities Press.

  • Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bakhtin, M. M. (1986). Speech genres and other late essays. University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barwise, J., & Moss, L. S. (1996). Vicious Circles: On the mathematics of non-Wellfounded phenomena. CSLI Publications.

  • Barwise, J., & Perry, J. (1983). Situations and attitudes. MIT Press, Bradford Books

  • Bavelas, J., & Chovil, N. (2000). Visible acts of meaning: An integrated message model of language in face-to-face dialogue. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 19(2), 163–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bhatia, V. K. (2004). Worlds of written discourse: A genre-based view. Bloomsbury Publishing.

  • Breitholtz, E., & Cooper, R. (2018). Towards a conversational game theory. Presented at Sociolinguistic, Psycholinguistic and Formal Perspectives on Meaning. https://sites.google.com/site/spfmeaning/home

  • Burnett, H. (2017). Sociolinguistic interaction and identity construction: The view from game-theoretic pragmatics. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 21(2), 238–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, P. S., & Long, B. E. L. (1976). Doctors talking to patients. A study of the verbal behaviour of general practitioners consulting in their surgeries. London, Her Majesty’s Stationery Office.

  • Carpenter, B. (1992). The logic of typed feature structures: With applications to unification grammars, logic programs, and constraint resolution. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ciardelli, I., Groenendijk, J., & Roelofsen, F. (2018). Inquisitive semantics. Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Clayman, S. E. (2013). Conversation analysis in the news interview. In J. Sidnell & F. Stivers (Eds.), The handbook of conversational analysis (pp. 630–656). Wiley-Blackwell.

  • Clayman, S., & Heritage, J. (2002). The news interview: Journalists and public figures on the air. Cambridge University Press.

  • Cohen, P., & Perrault, R. (1979). Elements of a plan-based theory of speech acts. Cognitive Science, 3, 177–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, R. (2005). Austinian truth, attitudes and type theory. Research on Language and Computation, 3(4), 333–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, R. (2012). Type theory and semantics in flux. In R. Kempson, N. Asher, & T. Fernando (Eds.), Handbook of the philosophy of science (Philosphy of Linguistics 14) (pp. 271–323). Elsevier.

  • Cooper, R. (2023). From perception to communication: An analysis of meaning and action using a theory of types with records (TTR). Oxford University Press.

  • Cooper, R., & Ginzburg, J. (2015). Type theory with records for natural language semantics. In C. Fox & S. Lappin (Eds.), Handbook of contemporary semantic theory, 2nd edition (pp. 375–407). Blackwell.

  • Cooper, R., & Poesio, M. (1994). Situation theory. In Fracas Deliverable D8. Centre for Cognitive Science, The Fracas Consortium.

  • Eades, D. (2000). I don’t think it’s an answer to the question: Silencing aboriginal witnesses in court. Language in Society, 29(2), 161–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehrlich, S., & Sidnell, J. (2006). I think that’s not an assumption you ought to make: Challenging presuppositions in inquiry testimony. Language in Society, 35(5), 655–676.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernández, R. (2006). Non-sentential utterances in dialogue: Classification, resolution and use. Ph.D. thesis, King’s College.

  • Fernando, T. (2007). Observing events and situations in time. Linguistics and Philosophy, 30(5), 527–550.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, R. (2013). Conversation analysis in the classroom. In J. Sidnell & F. Stivers (Eds.), The handbook of conversation analysis (pp. 593–610). Wiley-Blackwell.

  • Garrod, S., & Doherty, G. (1994). Conversation, co-ordination and convention: An empirical investigation of how groups establish linguistic conventions. Cognition, 53(3), 181–215.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gerstenberg, A., & Skupien-Dekens, C. (2021). A grammar of authority?–Directive speech acts and terms of address in two single-genre corpora of classical French. Journal of Historical Pragmatics, 22(1), 1–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gill, V. T., & Roberts, F. (2013). Coversation analysis in medicine. In J. Sidnell & F. Stivers (Eds.), The handbook of conversation analysis (pp. 575–592). Wiley-Blackwell.

  • Ginzburg, J. (1994). An update semantics for dialogue. In H. Bunt (Ed.), Proceedings of the 1st International Workshop on Computational Semantics. ITK, Tilburg University.

  • Ginzburg, J., & Lücking, A. (2020). On laughter and forgetting and reconversing: A neurologically-inspired model of conversational context. In S. Malamud, J. Pustejovsky, & J. Ginzburg (Eds.), Proceedings of SemDial 2020 (pp. 53–62). Brandeis University.

  • Ginzburg, J., & Sag, I. A. (2000). Interrogative investigations: The form, meaning and use of English interrogatives (CSLI Lecture Notes 124). CSLI Publications.

  • Ginzburg, J., Cooper, R., & Fernando, T. (2014a). Propositions, questions, and adjectives: A rich type theoretic approach. In Proceedings of the EACL 2014 workshop on type theory and natural language semantics (TTNLS) (pp. 89–96). Association for Computational Linguistics.

  • Ginzburg, J., Fernández, R., & Schlangen, D. (2014b). Disfluencies as intra-utterance dialogue moves. Semantics and Pragmatics, 7(9), 1–64.

  • Ginzburg, J. (2012). The interactive stance: Meaning for conversation. Oxford University Press.

  • Ginzburg, J. (2016). The semantics of dialogue. In M. Aloni & P. Dekker (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of formal semantics (pp. 130–170). Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ginzburg, J., & Fernández, R. (2010). Computational models of dialogue. In A. Clark, C. Fox, & S. Lappin (Eds.), Handbook of computational linguistics and natural language (pp. 429–481). Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ginzburg, J., Mazzocconi, C., & Tian, Y. (2020). Laughter as language. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 5(1), 104.

  • Goffman, E. (1974). Frame analysis: An essay on the organization of experience. Harvard University Press.

  • Greatbatch, D. (1988). A turn-taking system for British news interviews. Language in Society, 17(3), 401–430.

  • Groenendijk, J., & Stokhof, M. (1997). Questions. In J. van Benthem & A. ter Meulen (Eds.), Handbook of logic and linguistics (pp. 1055–1124). North Holland.

  • Grosz, B., & Sidner, C. L. (1986). Attention, intentions, and the structure of discourse. Computational Linguistics, 12, 175–204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Han, D., Tohti, T., & Hamdulla, A. (2022). Attention-based transformer-BiGRU for question classification. Information, 13(5), 214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Healey, P., & Mills, G. (2006). Participation, precedence and co-ordination in dialogue. In R. Sun & N. Miyake (Eds.), Proceedings of Cogsci06: The 28th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1470–1475). Psychology Press.

  • Heath, C. (2012). The dynamics of auction: Social interaction and the sale of fine art and antiques. Cambridge University Press.

  • Heath, C., & Luff, P. (2007a). Gesture and institutional interaction: Figuring bids in auctions of fine art and antiques. Gesture, 7(2), 215–240.

  • Heath, C., & Luff, P. (2007b). Ordering competition: The interactional accomplishment of the sale of art and antiques at auction. The British Journal of Sociology, 58(1), 63–85.

  • Heritage, J., & Greatbatch, D. (1989). On the institutional character of institutional talk: The case of news interviews. In P. A. Forsdrop (Ed.), Discours in professional and everyday culture (pp. 47–98). Department of Communication Studies, University of Linköping

  • Heritage, J., & Clayman, S. (2011). Talk in action: Interactions, identities, and institutions. Wiley.

  • Heritage, J., & Maynard, D. W. (2006). Communication in medical care: Interaction between primary care physicians and patients. Cambridge University Press.

  • Hoffmann, S., Evert, S., Smith, N., Lee, D., & Berglund-Prytz, Y. (2008). Corpus linguistics with BNCweb—A practical guide. Peter Lang.

  • Hymes, D. (1972). On communicative competence. In J.B. Pride & J. Holmes (Eds.), Sociolinguistics (pp.269–285). Penguin Books.

  • Kendon, A. (2004). Gesture: Visible action as utterance. Cambridge University Press.

  • Komter, M. (2013). Conversation analysis in the courtroom. In J. Sidnell & F. Stivers (Eds.), The handbook of conversation analysis (pp. 612–629). Wiley-Blackwell.

  • Krifka, M. (2001). For a structured meaning account of questions and answers. In C. Féry & W. Sternefeld (Eds.), Audiatur Vox Sapientiae. A Festschrift for Arnim von Stechow (pp. 287–319). Akademie Verlag.

  • Lam, P. (2009). The making of a BNC customised spoken corpus for comparative purposes. Corpora, 4(2), 167–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laplace, P. S. (1825). Essai philosophique sur les probabilités. Bachelier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larsson, S. (2002). Issue based dialogue management. Ph.D. thesis, Gothenburg University.

  • Larsson, S., & Berman, A. (2016). Domain-specific and general syntax and semantics in the talkamatic dialogue manager. Empirical Issues in Syntax and Semantics, 11, 91–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, D. (2002). Genres, registers, text types, domains and styles: Clarifying the concepts and navigating a path through the BNC jungle. In Teaching and learning by doing corpus analysis (pp. 245–292). Brill Rodopi.

  • Levinson, S. C. (1979). Activity types and language. Linguistics, 17(5–6), 365–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, X., & Roth, D. (2006). Learning question classifiers: The role of semantic information. Natural Language Engineering, 12(3), 229–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Madabushi, H. T., & Lee, M. (2016). High accuracy rule-based question classification using question syntax and semantics. In Proceedings of COLING 2016, the 26th international conference on computational linguistics: Technical Papers (pp. 1220–1230).

  • McCarthy, M. (2000). Mutually captive audiences: Small talk and the genre of close-contact service encounters. In J. Coupland (Ed.), Small talk (pp. 84–109). Pearson.

  • McHoul, A. (1978). The organization of turns at formal talk in the classroom. Language in Society, 7(2), 183–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McHoul, A. W. (1990). The organization of repair in classroom talk. Language in Society, 19(3), 349–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mehan, H. (1979). Learning lessons. Harvard University Press.

  • Mitchell, T. F. (1957). The language of buying and selling in Cyrenaica: A situational statement. Emile Larose.

  • Mundy, P., & Newell, L. (2007). Attention, joint attention, and social cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(5), 269–274. PMID: 19343102.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Osborne, M. J., & Rubinstein, A. (1994). A course in game theory. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pollard, C., & Sag, I. A. (1994). Head driven phrase structure grammar. University of Chicago Press.

  • Purver, M. (2006). Clarie: Handling clarification requests in a dialogue system. Research on Language & Computation, 4(2), 259–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ranta, A. (1994). Type theoretical grammar. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Revalski, J. P. (2003). The Banach-Mazur game: History and recent developments. Bulgarian Academy of Sciences, Institute of Mathematics and Informatics.

  • Robinson, J. D. (2003). An interactional structure of medical activities during acute visits and its implications for patients’ participation. Health Communication, 15(1), 27–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sag, I. A., Wasow, T., & Bender, E. (2003). Syntactic theory: A formal introduction (2nd ed.). CSLI Publications.

  • Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. (1977). Scripts, goals, plans, and understanding. Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ventola, E. (1987). The structure of social interaction: A systemic approach to the semiotics of service encounters. Pinter Pub Ltd.

  • Wiśniewski, A. (2015). Semantics of questions. In C. Fox & S. Lappin (Eds.), Handbook of contemporary semantic theory, 2nd edition (pp. 273–313). Blackwell.

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations. Transl. by G.E.M. Anscombe. Basil Blackwell. [Citations from second edition, 1988 reprint].

  • Wong, K.-C., & Ginzburg, J. (2018). Conversational types: A topological perspective. In L. Prévot, M. Ochs, & B. Favre (Eds.), Proceedings of SemDial 2018 (pp. 156–166). Aix en Provence.

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Paul Dekker, Susanne Trissler, and three anonymous reviewers for Linguistics and Philosophy for very helpful comments which have improved the paper significantly. Portions of the paper have been presented at seminars at CLASP, Gothenburg, ZAS, Berlin and at Université Paris Cité. We thanks participants at those venues for their useful comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jonathan Ginzburg.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

All authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Order of authorship is alphabetical. This work is supported by a public grant overseen by the French National Research Agency (ANR) as part of the program “Investissements d’Avenir” (reference: ANR-10-LABX-0083). It contributes to the IdEx Université de Paris - ANR-18-IDEX-0001. We also acknowledge a senior fellowship from the Institut Universitaire de France to the alphabetically first author.

Appendix: Basic Notions of TTR

Appendix: Basic Notions of TTR

Fundamental notions of TTR (Cooper, 2012, 2023) are the notion of a judgement and the notion of a record.

  • The typing judgement a : T classifies an object a as being of type T.

  • Records: a record is a set of ordered pairs of labels and objects of the form (58a), with the requirement that any label occur only once as the first element of one of the fields. A concrete instance is exemplified in (58b). Records are used here to model events and states, including utterances and dialogue gameboards.Footnote 24

figure bd
  • Record types: a record type is simply a record where each field represents a judgement rather than an assignment, as in (59).

figure be

The basic relationship between records and record types is that a record r is of type RT if each value in r assigned to a given label \(l_i\) satisfies the typing constraints imposed by RT on \(l_i\). More precisely,

figure bf

To exemplify this, (61a) is a possible type for (58b), assuming the conditions in (61b) hold. Record types are used to model utterance types (Saussurean/Formal Grammar signs) and to express rules of conversational interaction.

figure bg

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ginzburg, J., Wong, KC. Language games and their types. Linguist and Philos 47, 149–189 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10988-023-09393-2

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10988-023-09393-2

Keywords

Navigation