Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Gender and competition in adolescence: task matters

  • Published:
Experimental Economics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We look at gender differences among adolescents in Sweden in preferences for competition, altruism and risk. For competitiveness, we explore two different tasks that differ in associated stereotypes. We find no gender difference in competitiveness when comparing performance under competition to that without competition. We further find that boys and girls are equally likely to self-select into competition in a verbal task, but that boys are significantly more likely to choose to compete in a mathematical task. This gender gap diminishes and becomes non-significant when we control for actual performance, beliefs about relative performance, and risk preferences, or for beliefs only. Girls are also more altruistic and less risk taking than boys.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In particular, this might give rise to segregation in secondary education and consequently segregation in college or university education.

  2. Boys perform on average significantly better in the math task across all classes. From an optimality perspective though, the share of girls choosing to compete should not be smaller than that of boys. This is due to the fact that the gender composition of classes varies a lot and depends on academic specialization. Some classes therefore contain a large majority of girls or boys, and this is mirrored among the top performers in each class.

  3. Among the participants, 56 attended the first year (57 % girls), 95 the second (51 % girls) and 50 the third year in high school (50 % girls). 15 students attended a mixed class with students from years 1 and 2 (47 % girls). For these students we have no information on which year they actually attended at the moment of the study.

  4. Participants earned on average about SEK 25. In comparison to previous studies on adults these incentives are small (see for example Niederle and Vesterlund 2007), while in comparison to similar studies on children the incentives are more important (see for example Cárdenas et al. 2012).

  5. When constructing this comparison group we made a random draw with replacement for each participant separately. This implied that a participant could be drawn for comparison more than once.

  6. In one of our previous studies in Sweden, the math task has been rated as more boyish and the word task as more girlish by both genders (Cárdenas et al. 2012). Moreover, vocabulary knowledge is positively correlated with reading comprehension (Yovanoff et al. 2005).

  7. The charity was the Swedish section of “Save the children”. We chose Save the Children since it is a large and well-established NGO in Sweden. Even if Save the Children does not explicitly focus or work on gender issues, there could be gender differences in perceptions of the charity.

  8. We collected a variety of variables (the full survey is available from the authors on request). In this paper we use class (year), birth month, height, GPA, and life satisfaction (scale 0–10).

  9. We also perform a regression analysis for each behavior using the control variables from the survey (class year, birth month, height, GPA, and life satisfaction). The female coefficient remains similar for all behaviors except altruism, where it is no longer significant. Most control variables are not significant. For those that are significant, we find the following correlations: altruism is negatively correlated with birth month (individuals born early are more altruistic) whereas it is positively correlated with class year and GPA. Risk taking is positively correlated with class year. Competitiveness as measured by the choice to compete in the verbal task is negatively correlated with GPA. For competitive performance change we also conduct a quantile regression on absolute performance change and find no gender gap in math or word search in any part of the performance distribution.

  10. Given that the gender gap in math performance in Sweden is small compared to many other countries (Guiso et al. 2008) this is a somewhat puzzling result. However, it might have to do with the specific sample of schools in our study.

  11. One participant did not choose payment scheme for the third stage in math, and two did not perform in this stage. In the word task, two participants did not choose a payment scheme. When possible, these individuals are included in the analysis. Including or excluding these participants has no effect on the results.

  12. A sample size analysis indicates that 2037 observations would be needed to obtain a significant result for the gender gap in competition choice in word search. The basis for the power calculation is a significance level of 5 % and a power of 80 %.

  13. A t-test indicates that boys are significantly under-confident in math (p=0.041).

  14. Actual performance and beliefs about performance in the regression analysis is based on performance and relative performance beliefs in the second stage (the tournament). Using performance in the third stage instead of performance in the second stage does not qualitatively change our gender results. Since 14 participants were inconsistent in their choices in the risk task, the risk measure included here is the number of risky choices the participants make.

  15. Including interaction variables between female and performance and female and performance beliefs do not provide further insights; the results do not alter.

  16. A correlation analysis between all the behaviors we examine also shows that altruism is positively related to risk taking (as number of risky choices) in the incentivized risk task (p<0.001), but not in the self-reported question. We also find no correlation between altruism and competitive choices (p=0.255 for math and p=0.479 for word). A similar pattern is found among boys and girls separately. See Appendix Table 8. Our regression results do not change if we include altruism as a control variable.

  17. The result is qualitatively similar when analyzing the number of risky choices instead of the switching point in order to include individuals that switch back and forth between the lottery and the safe points. Girls are still less risk taking compared to boys (p=0.007). Moreover, there is no gender difference in the variance of the incentivized risk taking variable (p=0.210).

  18. Matrilocality often refers to a society in which a married couple lives with or close to the wife’s parents.

References

  • Almås, I., Cappelen, A. W., Salvanes, K. G., Sørensen, E., & Tungodden, B. (2012). Willingness to compete: family matters. Department of Economics, NHH, Discussion paper, 23/2012.

  • Andersen, S., Ertac, S., Gneezy, U., List, J., Maximiano, & Gender, S. F. (2013). Competitiveness and socialization at a young age: evidence from a matrilineal and a patriarchal society. Review of Economics and Statistics.

  • Apicella, C. L., Dreber, A., Campbell, B., Gray, P., Hoffman, M., & Little, A. C. (2008). Testosterone and financial risk-taking. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 385–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S. L., Gunderson, E. A., Ramirez, G., & Levine, S. C. (2010). Female teachers’ math anxiety affects girls’ math achievement. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(5), 1860–1863.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benenson, J. F., Pascoe, J., & Radmore, N. (2007). Children’s altruistic behavior in the dictator game. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28(3), 168–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bertrand, M. (2010). In New perspectives on gender. Handbook of labor economics, vol. 4, (Part b) (pp. 1543–1590). Chap. 17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blake, P. R., & Rand, D. G. (2010). Currency value moderates equity preference among young children. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 210–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Booth, A. L., & Nolen, P. J. (2012a). Choosing to compete: how different are girls and boys? Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 81(2).

  • Booth, A. L., & Nolen, P. J. (2012b). Gender differences in risk behaviour: does nurture matter? Economic Journal, 122(558), F56–F78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bosch-Domènech, A., & Silvestre, J. (2012). Measuring risk aversion with lists: a new bias. Barcelona GSE Working Paper Series, Working Paper #634.

  • Borghans, L., Golsteyn, B. H. H., Heckman, J. J., & Meijers, H. (2013). Gender differences in risk aversion and ambiguity aversion. Journal of the European Economic Association, 7(2–3), 649–658.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boschini, A., Dreber, A., von Essen, E., Muren, A., & Ranehill, E. (2012). Gender and preferences: a study on a simple random sample of the Swedish population. Working paper.

  • Brañas-Garza, P., & Rustichini, A. (2011). Organizing effects of testosterone and economic behavior: not just risk taking. PLoS ONE 6(12).

  • Buser, T. (2012). The impact of the menstrual cycle and hormonal contraceptives on competitiveness. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 83(1), 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buser, T., Niederle, M., & Oosterbeek, H. (2012). Gender, competitiveness and career choices. NBER Working Paper Series, w18576.

  • Cárdenas, J.-C., Dreber, A., von Essen, E., & Ranehill, E. (2012). Gender differences in competitiveness and risk taking: comparing children in Colombia and Sweden. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 83(1), 11–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charles, M., & Grusky, D. B. (2004). Occupational ghettos: the worldwide segregation of women and men. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Croson, R., & Gneezy, U. (2009). Gender differences in preferences. Journal of Economic Literature, 47(2), 1–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cvencek, D., Meltzoff, A. N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2011). Math–gender stereotypes in elementary school children. Child Development, 82(3), 766–779.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dahlbom, L., Jakobsson, A., Jakobsson, N., & Kotsadam, A. (2011). Gender and overconfidence: are girls really overconfident? Applied Economics Letters, 18(4), 325–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Datta Gupta, N., Poulsen, A., & Villeval, M.-C. (2011). Gender matching and competitiveness: experimental evidence. Economic Inquiry, 51(1), 816–835.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Huffman, D., Sunde, U., Schupp, J., & Wagner, G. G. (2011). Individual risk attitudes: measurement, determinants and behavioral consequences. Journal of the European Economic Association, 9(3), 522–550.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dreber, A., von Essen, E., & Ranehill, E. (2011). Outrunning the gender gap: boys and girls compete equally. Experimental Economics, 14(4), 567–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eckel, C., & Grossman, P. (1996). Altruism in anonymous dictator games. Games and Economic Behavior, 16, 181–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eckel, C., & Grossman, P. (2008a). Men, women and risk aversion: experimental evidence. In C. Plott & V. Smith (Eds.), Handbook on experimental economics results (pp. 1063–1071). New York: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eckel, C., & Grossman, P. (2008b). Differences in the economic decisions of men and women: experimental evidence. In C. Plott & V. Smith (Eds.), Handbook on experimental economics results (pp. 509–519). New York: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Eckel, C. C., Grossman, P. J., Johnson, C. A., de Oliveira, A. C. M., Rojas, C., & Wilson, R. K. (2011). Social norms of sharing in high school: teen giving in the dictator game. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 80(3), 603–612.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eckel, C. C., Grossman, P. J., Johnson, C. A., de Oliveira, A. C. M., Rojas, C., & Wilson, R. K. (2012). School environment and risk preferences: experimental evidence. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 45(3), 265–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engel, C. (2011). Dictator games: a meta study. Experimental Economics, 14(4), 583–610.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Favara, M. (2012). The cost of acting “girly”: gender stereotypes and educational choices. IZA Discussion Paper No. 7037.

  • Fehr-Duda, H., de Gennaro, M., & Schubert, R. (2006). Gender, financial risk, and probability weights. Theory and Decision, 60(2–3), 283–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flory, J., Leibbrandt, A., & List, J. (2010). Do competitive work places deter female workers? A large-scale natural field experiment on gender differences in job-entry decisions. NBER Working Paper Series, w16546.

  • Gneezy, U., & Rustichini, A. (2004). Gender and competition at a young age. The American Economic Review, 94(2), 377–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gneezy, U., Niederle, M., & Rustichini, A. (2003). Performance in competitive environments: gender differences. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 118(3), 1049–1074.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gong, B., & Yang, C.-L. (2012). Gender differences in risk attitudes: field experiments on the Matrilineal Mosuo and the Patriarchal Yi. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 83(1), 59–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grosse, N. D., & Reiner, G. (2010). Explaining gender differences in competitiveness: gender-task stereotypes. Jena Economic Research Papers, 2010–017.

  • Guiso, L., Monte, F., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L. (2008). Culture, Gender, and Math-Science, 320(2880), 1164–1165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gummerum, M., Hanoch, Y., Keller, M., Parsons, K., & Hummel, A. (2010). Preschoolers‘ allocations in the dictator game: the role of moral emotions. Journal of Economic Psychology, 31(1), 25–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Günther, C., Arslan Ekinici, N., Schwieren, C., & Strobel, M. (2009). Women can’t jump?—An experiment on competitive attitudes and stereotype threat. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 75(3), 395–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harbaugh, W. T., Krause, K., & Vesterlund, L. (2002). Risk attitudes of children and adults: choices over small and large probability gains and losses. Experimental Economics, 5(1), 53–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harbaugh, W. T., Krause, K., & Liday, S. G. (2003). Bargaining by children. Unpublished manuscript, University of Oregon.

  • Hausmann, R., Tyson, L., & Zahidi, S. (2010). The global gender gap report. World Economic Forum. Geneve.

  • Joensen, J. S., & Nielsen, H. S. (2013). Math and gender: is math a route to a high-powered career? IZA Discussion Paper 7164.

  • Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2005). Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature, 435, 673–676.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, U., Wozniak, D., Davidson, C., Kuhns, D., & Harbaugh, B. (2012). Competitiveness across the life span: the feisty fifties. Psychology and Aging, 27(2), 278–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nosek, B. A., & Smyth, F. L. (2011). Implicit social cognitions predict sex differences in math engagement and achievement. American Educational Research Journal, 48(5), 1125–1156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niederle, M., & Vesterlund, L. (2007). Do women shy away from competition? Do men compete too much? The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 122(3), 1067–1101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niederle, M., & Vesterlund, L. (2010). Explaining the gender gap in math test scores: the role of competition. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 24(2), 129–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niederle, M., & Yestrumskas, A. H. (2008). Gender differences in seeking challenges: the role of institutions. NBER Working Paper Series, w13922.

  • Örs, E., Frederic, P., & Eloic, P. (2008). Performance gender-gap: does competition matter? CEPR Working Paper 6891.

  • Sapienza, P., Zingales, L., & Maestripieri, D. (2009). Gender differences in financial risk aversion and career choices are affected by testosterone. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 15268–15273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Savikhin, A. (2011). Is there a gender gap in preschoolers’ competitiveness? An experiment in the U.S. Mimeo.

  • Schipper, B. C. (2012). Sex hormones and choice under risk. Mimeo.

  • Shurchkov, O. (2012). Under pressure: gender differences in output quality and quantity under competition and time constraints. Journal of the European Economic Association, 10(5), 1189–1213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sutter, M., & Rützler, D. (2010). Gender differences in competition emerge early in life. IZA Discussion Paper 5015.

  • Sutter, M., Kocher, M. G., Rützler, D., & Trautmann, S. T. (2013). Impatience and uncertainty: experimental decisions predict adolescents’ field behavior. The American Economic Review, 103(1), 510–531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wozniak, D., Harbaugh, W., & Mayr, U. (2010). Choices about competition: differences by gender and hormonal fluctuations, and the role of relative performance feedback. MPRA Paper 21097.

  • Yovanoff, P., Duesbery, L., Alonzo, J., & Tindal, G. (2005). Grade-level invariance of a theoretical causal structure predicting reading comprehension with vocabulary and oral reading fluency. Educational Measurement, Issues and Practice, 24(3), 4–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zethraeus, N., Kocoska-Maras, L., Ellingsen, T., von Schoultz, B., Lindén Hirschberg, A., & Johannesson, M. (2009). A randomized trial of the effect of estrogen and testosterone on economic behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 6535–6538.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, J. (2010). Do girls in China compete just as much as boys? Evidence from an experiment that predicts educational choice. Mimeo.

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful for comments from Johan Almenberg, Simon Gächter, Uri Gneezy, Magnus Johannesson, Christoph Mathys, Astri Muren, Robert Östling, David G. Rand, Roberto Weber, the editor David Cooper, two anonymous referees, and seminar participants at WAPPP at the Harvard Kennedy School, MOVE Workshop on Gender Differences in Competitiveness and Risk Taking, Stockholm School of Economics and Stockholm University, as well as help with the data collection from Aron Backström, Peter Gerlach and Karin Hederos Eriksson. Financial support from the Jan Wallander and Tom Hedelius Foundation, the Swedish Council for Working Life and Social Research (FAS) and the Carl Silfvén Foundation is gratefully acknowledged.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Eva Ranehill.

Electronic Supplementary Material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

(DOCX 55 kB)

Appendix

Appendix

These are example exercises, of a similar type as the participants encountered during the study.

Example math exercise: 10+83+56=___________

Example word puzzle:

H

U

N

D

E

F

E

T

A

T

Y

O

U

Å

T

S

T

B

M

V

T

O

R

Å

S

O

I

A

O

O

Table 8 Correlation matrix between altruism, risk preferences and competitiveness

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Dreber, A., von Essen, E. & Ranehill, E. Gender and competition in adolescence: task matters. Exp Econ 17, 154–172 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-013-9361-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-013-9361-0

Keywords

JEL Classification

Navigation