Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

A Mind with a Mind of Its Own: How Complexity Theory Can Inform Early Science Pedagogy

  • REFLECTION ON THE FIELD
  • Published:
Educational Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In the current paper, we develop an approach to early science pedagogy that is based on insights about how complex adaptive systems function. Complexity approaches have an important advantage over traditional information-processing approaches: They anticipate the proverbial ‘mind with a mind of its own’ without having to postulate exclusively mental constructs. They also offer insights about key determinants of learning and effective pedagogy, again without postulating exclusively mental constructs. For complex adaptive systems, learning depends on the presence of sufficiently salient novelty (i.e., variability), and it depends on the presence of sufficiently salient repetitions or ordered patterns (i.e., stability). Science learning, therefore, requires science-relevant novelty and science-relevant patterns of order. Equipped with these insights, we address two challenges of early science pedagogy: (1) how to combine children’s self-guided explorations with teachers’ strategic interventions, and (2) how to minimize the chances of generating misconceptions about science. The answer lies in creating a learning context that maximizes science-relevant variability and science-relevant stability. If both aspects are abundantly available, a child’s self-guided explorations are effective. Conversely, if either aspect is missing, efforts must be made to add them strategically to a child’s experience. Adding science-relevant stability is particularly challenging, yet crucial to avoid science misconceptions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The idea of elements being changed is broadly conceived to include not only genuine change but also the idea of elements being created. Incidentally, education typically focuses the process of creating something (e.g., knowledge), while ecology typically focuses on the process of change (e.g., adaptation of species). However, the distinction between creating and changing is likely to be artificial. In reality, elements of complex adaptive systems are neither fully new, nor fully old.

  2. In order to explain why some words are learned much faster than others, Rosch (1975) focused on a word’s level of specificity. Basic-level concepts were said to be neither too specific nor too abstract. Sub-ordinate level concepts, on the other hand, were said to be more specific than basic-level categories; and super-ordinate level categories were said to be less specific (i.e., more abstract) than basic-level categories. The complexity view is in line with this taxonomy but adds detail to what is meant with specificity.

References

  • Allesina, S., & Bondavalli, C. (2003). Steady state of ecosystem flow networks: a comparison between balancing procedures. Ecological Modelling, 165(2-3), 221–229.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (1996). ACT: a simple theory of complex cognition. American Psychologist, 51(4), 355–365.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (2015). Cognitive psychology and its implications. New York: Worth Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashiabi, G. S. (2007). Play in the preschool classroom: its socioemotional significance and the teacher’s role in play. Early Childhood Education Journal, 35(2), 199–207.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barab, S. A., Cherkes-Julkowski, M., Swenson, R., Garrett, S., Shaw, R. E., & Young, M. (1999). Principles of self-organization: learning as participation in autocatakinetic systems. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 8(3-4), 349–390.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barabási, A.-L., & Albert, R. (1999). Emergence of scaling in random networks. Science, 286(5439), 509–512.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59(1), 617–645.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brenneman, K., & Louro, I. F. (2008). Science journals in the preschool classroom. Early Childhood Education Journal, 36(2), 113–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bush, V. (1945). As we may think. The Atlantic Monthly, 176, 101–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caldarelli, G., & Catanzaro, M. (2012). Networks: a very short introduction (Vol. 335). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (2000). Science education as conceptual change. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 21(1), 13–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, B. (2014). How we learn: the surprising truth about when, where, and why it happens. New York, NY: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Castillo, R., Kloos, H., Richardson, M., & Waltzer, T. (2015). Beliefs as self-sustaining networks: drawing parallels between networks of ecosystems and adults’ predictions. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chow, J. Y., Davids, K., Hristovski, R., Araújo, D., & Passos, P. (2011). Nonlinear pedagogy: learning design for self-organizing neurobiological systems. New Ideas in Psychology, 29(2), 189–200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, A., & Collins, S. (2007). Complexity science and student teacher supervision. Teaching and Teacher Education, 23(2), 160–172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2006.10.006.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colunga, E., & Smith, L. B. (2008). Knowledge embedded in process: the self-organization of skilled noun learning. Developmental Science, 11(2), 195–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, C., Goodman, N. D., & Schulz, L. E. (2011). Where science starts: spontaneous experiments in preschoolers’ exploratory play. Cognition, 120(3), 341–349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, B., & Simmt, E. (2003). Understanding learning systems: mathematics education and complexity science. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 34(2), 137. https://doi.org/10.2307/30034903.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, B., & Sumara, D. (2006). Complexity and education: inquiries into learning, teaching and research. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, C., Nikolić, I., & Dijkema, G. P. (2009). Integration of life cycle assessment into agent-based modeling: toward informed decisions on evolving infrastructure systems. Journal of Industrial Ecology, 13(2), 306–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, T. W. (2011). Incomplete nature: how mind emerged from matter. New York: WW Norton & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A. (1985). Development of the ability to use recall to guide action, as indicated by infants’ performance on A-not-B. Child Development, 56(4), 868–883.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A. (1990). Developmental time course in human infants and infant monkeys, and the neural bases of inhibitory control in reaching. In A. Diamond (Ed.), The development and neural bases of higher cognitive functions (pp. 637–676). New York: National Academic of Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fenwick, T. (2003). Reclaiming and re-embodying experiential learning through complexity science. Studies in the Education of Adults, 35(2), 123–141. https://doi.org/10.1080/02660830.2003.11661478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fenwick, T. (2008). Responsibility, complexity science and education: dilemmas and uncertain responses. Studies in Philosophy and Education, 28(2), 101–118. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11217-008-9099-x.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fine, C. (2008). A mind of its own: how your brain distorts and deceives. New York: WW Norton & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, K. R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Newcombe, N., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2013). Taking shape: Supporting preschoolers’ acquisition of geometric knowledge through guided play. Child Development, 84(6), 1872–1878.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., & Flavell, E. R. (1998). The mind has a mind of its own: developing knowledge about mental uncontrollability. Cognitive Development, 13(1), 127–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fleer, M. (1991). Socially constructed learning in early childhood science education. Research in Science Education, 21(1), 96–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fleer, M., & Beasley, W. (1991). A study of conceptual development in early childhood. Research in Science Education, 21(1), 104–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1981). Representations: philosophical essays on the foundations of cognitive science. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fullan, M. (1994). Coordinating top-down and bottom-up strategies for education reform. In R. F. Elmore & S. H. Fuhrman (Eds.), The governance of curriculum: Yearbook of the Association for Supervision and Curriculum Development. Alexandria, VA: Association for the Supervision of Curriculum and Development.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gargiulo, M., & Benassi, M. (2000). Trapped in your own net? Network cohesion, structural holes, and the adaptation of social capital. Organization Science, 11(2), 183–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga, M. S. (2004). The cognitive neurosciences. Cambridge: MIT press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs, R. W. (2005). Embodiment and cognitive science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gobert, J. D., & Buckley, B. C. (2000). Introduction to model-based in teaching and learning in science education. International Journal of Science Education, 22(9), 891–894.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golbeck, S. L. (2001). Psychological perspectives on early childhood education. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A. (1996). The scientist as child. Philosophy of Science, 63(4), 485–514.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A. (2012). Scientific thinking in young children: theoretical advances, empirical research, and policy implications. Science, 337(6102), 1623–1627.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gross, T., & Blasius, B. (2008). Adaptive coevolutionary networks: a review. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 5(20), 259–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hadley, E. (2002). Playful disruptions. Early Years, 22(1), 9–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halford, G. S., Wilson, W. H., & Phillips, S. (1998). Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: Implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 803–831.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilpert, J. C., & Marchand, G. C. (2018). Complex systems research in educational psychology: aligning theory and method. Educational Psychologist, 53(3), 185–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holland, J. H. (2000). Emergence: from chaos to order. Oxford: OUP Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holland, J. H. (2006). Studying complex adaptive systems. Journal of Systems Science and Complexity, 19(1), 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11424-0060001-z.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunter, J., Monroe-Ossi, H., & Fountain, C. (2008). Young Florida naturalists: concept mapping and science learning of preschool children. In A. J. Cañas, P. Reiske, M. Åhlberg, & D. Novak (Eds.), Concept maps: Connecting educators. Proceedings of the Third International Conference on Concept Mapping. Tallinn, Estonia & Helsinki, Finland: University of Finland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iberall, A. S., & McCulloch, W. S. (1969). The organizing principle of complex living systems. Journal of Basic Engineering, 91(2), 290–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobson, M. J., & Wilensky, U. (2006). Complex systems in education: scientific and educational importance and implications for the learning sciences. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 15(1), 11–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobson, M. J., Kapur, M., & Reimann, P. (2016). Conceptualizing debates in learning and educational research: toward a complex systems conceptual framework of learning. Educational Psychologist, 51(2), 210–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonassen, D. H., & Grabowski, B. L. (2012). Handbook of individual differences, learning, and instruction. Abingdon: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, S. L. (1990). Children’s play: the journey from theory to practice. In E. S. Klugman & S. Smilansky (Eds.), Children’s play and learning: perspectives and policy implications (pp. 173–187). New York: Teachers College Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: a developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge: MIT press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kauffmann, S. A. (1993). The origins of order: self-organization and selection in evolution. NY: Oxford University Press.

  • Kelemen, D. (1999). The scope of teleological thinking in preschool children. Cognition, 70(3), 241–272.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kello, C. T., Beltz, B. C., Holden, J. G., & Van Orden, G. C. (2007). The emergent coordination of cognitive function. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136(4), 551–568.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelso, J. S., Dumas, G., & Tognoli, E. (2013). Outline of a general theory of behavior and brain coordination. Neural Networks, 37, 120–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenyon, L., Schwarz, C., & Hug, B. (2008). The benefits of scientific modeling: Constructing, using, evaluating, and revising scientific models helps students advance their scientific ideas, learn to think critically, and understand the nature of science. Science and Children, 46, 40–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: an analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist, 41(2), 75–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kloos, H., Baker, H., Luken, E., Brown, R., Pfeiffer, D., & Carr, V. (2012). Preschoolers learning science: myth or reality? In H. Kloos, B. J. Morris, & J. L. Amaral (Eds.), Current topics in children’s learning and cognition (pp. 45–70). Rijeka: Tech - Open Access Publisher. https://doi.org/10.5772/54119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kloos, H., Waltzer, T., Maltbie, C., Brown, R., & Carr, V. (2018). Inconsistencies in early science education: can nature help streamline state standards? Ecopsychology, 10(4), 243–258.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D. (1989). Children and adults as intuitive scientists. Psychological Review, 96(4), 674–689.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kurby, C. A., & Zacks, J. M. (2008). Segmentation in the perception and memory of events. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(2), 72–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • La Cerra, P., & Bingham, R. (2002). The origin of minds: evolution, uniqueness, and the new science of the self. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. (2008). Women, fire, and dangerous things. Chicago: University of Chicago press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazonder, A. W., & Harmsen, R. (2016). Meta-analysis of inquiry-based learning: effects of guidance. Review of Educational Research, 86(3), 681–718. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654315627366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mason, M. (2008). Complexity theory and the philosophy of education. Complexity Theory and the Philosophy of Education, 40, 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444307351.ch1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. (2004). Should there be a three-strikes rule against pure discovery learning? The case for guided methods of instruction. American Psychologist, 59(1), 14–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mendes, J. F. F., & Dorogovtsev, S. N. (2003). Evolution of networks: from biological nets to the internet and WWW. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michaelian, K. (2005). Thermodynamic stability of ecosystems. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 237(3), 323–335.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63(2), 81–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Research Council. (2001). Eager to learn: educating our preschoolers. Washington, D.C.: National Academies Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicolis, G., & Prigogine, I. (1989). Exploring complexity: an introduction. New York: WH Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Novak, J. D. (2010). Learning, creating, and using knowledge: concept maps as facilitative tools in schools and corporations (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ohlsson, S. (2011). Deep learning: how the mind overrides experience. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ormrod, J. E. (2011). Human learning. London: Pearson Higher Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pantaleone, J. (2002). Synchronization of metronomes. American Journal of Physics, 70(10), 992–1000.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1969). The psychology of the child (Vol. 5001). New York City: Basic books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & DiGirolamo, G. J. (1998). Executive attention: conflict, target detection, and cognitive control. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rączaszek-Leonardi, J. (2016). How does a word become a message? An illustration on a developmental time-scale. New Ideas in Psychology, 42, 46–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riley, M. A., & Turvey, M. T. (2002). Variability and determinism in motor behavior. Journal of Motor Behavior, 34(2), 99–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104(3), 192–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 27–48). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schulz, L. (2012). The origins of inquiry: inductive inference and exploration in early childhood. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(7), 382–389.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shtulman, A. (2017). Scienceblind: why our intuitive theories about the world are so often wrong. London: Hachette UK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shtulman, A., & Valcarcel, J. (2012). Scientific knowledge suppresses but does not supplant earlier intuitions. Cognition, 124(2), 209–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simons, D. J., & Keil, F. C. (1995). An abstract to concrete shift in the development of biological thought: the insides story. Cognition, 56(2), 129–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, J., Thomas, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (2009). Toward a new unified theory of development: connectionism and dynamical systems theory re-considered. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sporns, O., & Zwi, J. D. (2004). The small world of the cerebral cortex. Neuroinformatics, 2(2), 145–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strogatz, S. (2004). Sync: the emerging science of spontaneous order. London: Penguin UK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swenson, R. (2000). Spontaneous order, autocatakinetic closure, and the development of space-time. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 901, 311–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tanaka, J. W., & Taylor, M. (1991). Object categories and expertise: is the basic level in the eye of the beholder? Cognitive Psychology, 23(3), 457–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tumer, E. C., & Brainard, M. S. (2007). Performance variability enables adaptive plasticity of ‘crystallized’ adult birdsong. Nature, 450(7173), 1240–1244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turvey, M. T., & Carello, C. (2012). On intelligence from first principles: guidelines for inquiry into the hypothesis of physical intelligence (PI). Ecological Psychology, 24, 3–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ulanowicz, R. E. (2009). A third window: natural life beyond Newton and Darwin. West Conshohocken: Templeton Foundation Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ulanowicz, R. E. (2012). Growth and development: ecosystems phenomenology. Berlin: Springer Science & Business Media.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Orden, G. C., Holden, J. G., & Turvey, M. T. (2003). Self-organization of cognitive performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(3), 331–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S. (2007). Conceptual change and education. Human Development, 50(1), 47–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1992). Mental models of the earth: a study of conceptual change in childhood. Cognitive Psychology, 24(4), 535–585.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weber, B. H. (2010). What is life? Defining life in the context of emergent complexity. Origins of Life and Evolution of Biospheres, 40(2), 221–229.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weisberg, R. W., & Reeves, L. M. (2013). Cognition: from memory to creativity. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitchurch, G. G., & Constantine, L. L. (2009). Systems theory. In P. Boss, W. J. Doherty, R. LaRossa, W. R. Schumm, & S. K. Steinmetz (Eds.), Sourcebook of family theories and methods (pp. 325–355). New York City: Springer US.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiser, M., & Smith, C. L. (2008). Learning and teaching about matter in grades K-8: when should the atomic-molecular theory be introduced. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), International handbook of research on conceptual change (pp. 205–239). Abingdon: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, C. (2000). The development of scientific reasoning skills. Developmental Review, 20(1), 99–149.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

Support for this work was provided by the National Science Foundation (DLS 13138890; Kloos).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Heidi Kloos.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Highlights

• A new approach to early science learning is offered, motivated by insights about the mind as a complex adaptive system.

• Complexity theory, unlike information-processing theory, anticipates the often-lamented tendency for the mind to behave as if having a mind of its own.

• Two key determinants of learning are identified: (1) the amount of variability in the surrounding, and (2) the amount of stability from one experience to the next.

• Science topics can be organized systematically by whether the variability and stability available in the surrounding are relevant to the chosen science topic.

• Science topics with sufficient variability and stability (i.e., basic-level science content) are best learned via mere explorations, without needing teacher intervention.

• Science topics that lack either variability or stability (i.e., sub-ordinate and super-ordinate level science content, respectively) require teacher intervention to support learning.

Initial impetus for this work came from inspiring interactions with preschool teachers during a workshop on science learning, organized by LeeAnn Lang and the Cincinnati Museum Center. We thank Stellan Ohlsson, Vicki Carr, Anna Fisher, Chris Erb, Chris Bell, and Jay Dixon for formative comments on earlier versions of this manuscript. Essential feedback was also provided by the students of the Special Topics graduate course in experimental psychology at the University of Cincinnati (Spring 2017). For questions about this manuscript, please contact the authors (heidi.kloos@uc.edu, bakerhe@live.com, or twaltzer@ucsc.edu).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kloos, H., Baker, H. & Waltzer, T. A Mind with a Mind of Its Own: How Complexity Theory Can Inform Early Science Pedagogy. Educ Psychol Rev 31, 735–752 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-019-09472-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-019-09472-6

Keywords

Navigation