College athletes face challenges beyond those of typical undergraduates in the United States (Kimball and Freysinger 2003). These include rigorous training and competitive schedules with minimal days off, frequent travel, external pressures to perform, difficulties with coaches and teammates, athletic and academic role conflict, and insufficient time to nurture non-sport relationships and activities (Broughton and Neyer 2001; Cosh and Tully 2015; Loudon et al. 2013; Settles et al. 2002; Watson and Kissinger 2007). Effectively regulating emotions that arise from daily stressors is crucial for optimal functioning (e.g. Min et al. 2013; Troy and Mauss 2011; Tugade and Fredrickson 2007). However, an overreliance on global trait measures of emotion regulation (e.g. Gross and John 2003; Uphill et al. 2012) and a predominant focus on sport-specific situations (e.g. Gaudreau and Blondin 2004a, b; Martinent et al. 2015; Poczwardowski and Conroy 2002) gives an incomplete picture of the regulatory strategies and stressors college athletes encounter in sport and life.

Numerous studies have explored strategies for regulating negative emotions, such as cognitive reappraisal (i.e. changing one’s thinking about a situation; e.g. Gross and John 2003; McRae et al. 2012), acceptance (i.e. mindfully acknowledging distressing emotions or situations without struggling to change them; e.g. Gratz and Tull 2010; Wolgast et al., 2011), problem-solving (e.g. Bell and D’Zurilla 2009), social support seeking (e.g. Marroquín 2011), and cognitive and behavioral avoidance (i.e., trying not to think or act in ways that exacerbate distress; Kashdan et al. 2006; Olatunji et al. 2010). Evidence suggests that certain strategies, such as cognitive reappraisal and acceptance, are associated with healthier emotional functioning, while strategies involving suppression or avoidance of distress are associated with emotional dysfunction (e.g. Brockman et al. 2017; Hofmann et al. 2009; Kuba and Scheibe 2017; Machell et al. 2015; Moore et al. 2008). Experience-sampling methods, such as smartphone-based daily diary and ecological momentary assessment (EMA), have been used to uncover the temporal dynamics, contextual correlates, and consequences of these regulatory strategies in the daily lives of healthy adults and clinical populations (e.g. Benson et al. 2019; Colombo et al. 2020; Gruber et al. 2013; Ludwig et al. 2020; Visser et al. 2018). Research suggests experience sampling methods mitigate recall bias when assessing momentary psychological states, including emotions (e.g. Ellison et al. 2020; Scollon et al. 2009).

More intensive experience sampling methods (e.g. EMA) have been used successfully to study athletes’ self-talk during sport performance (e.g. Dickens et al. 2018; Van Raalte et al. 2019), but no studies to our knowledge have tested the feasibility of EMA for studying emotion regulation in college athletes’ daily lives during their competitive seasons. Given college athletes’ demanding schedules, daily diary methods (i.e. recalling and reporting on daily experiences at the end of each day) have been more frequently used to capture college athletes’ psychological experiences in daily life (e.g. Riley et al. 2020; Shapiro et al. 2017; Shorey et al. 2014; Tamminen et al. 2019). Daily diaries demonstrate considerable yet imperfect agreement with real-time EMA measures when assessing daily emotions, with specific bias toward recalling stronger/more salient emotional experiences when thinking about the day as a whole (Neubauer et al. 2019). However, this bias makes daily diaries suitable for capturing particularly strong emotional experiences in daily life – both positive and negative – and associated emotion regulation strategies.

Only recently have researchers begun to explore the regulation of positive emotions related to daily positive events. Cross-sectional and longitudinal data suggest that savoring, a set of cognitive-behavioral strategies to upregulate positive emotions related to positive events (e.g. counting blessings, sharing with others, deeply processing sensory details), enhances positive emotions (e.g. Bryant and Veroff 2007; Jose et al. 2012; Silton et al. 2020; Sytine et al. 2019; Quoidbach et al., 2015). Consistent with savoring theory (e.g. Bryant and Veroff 2007), evidence from daily diary studies suggest that savoring moderates (strengthens) associations between positive events and momentary positive emotions (Jose et al. 2012). Interestingly, diary data also suggest that savoring moderates (buffers) the negative association between daily hassles and daily hope, optimism, and self-efficacy (Sytine et al. 2019), suggesting that savoring may promote healthy responses to daily stressors. Similarly, brief 1–2 week savoring interventions have been shown to downregulate negative emotions and enhance resilience (defined as the ability to bounce back from stressful experiences; Smith et al. 2008), at post-intervention (Hurley and Kwon 2013; McMakin et al. 2011) and three months later (Smith and Hanni 2019). With grueling schedules comprised of more “journeys” (training and practice) than “arrivals” (winning games or tournaments, individual accolades, etc.), savoring smaller daily achievements and positive events may be an overlooked strategy for enhancing college athletes’ emotional functioning in the face of daily stressors.

Despite an abundance of research on regulatory responses to negative emotions and experiences, there is still much to learn about effective emotion regulation strategies for college athletes in daily life. Given the culture of college sports in the United States, which prizes relentless efforts to improve, ameliorate deficiencies, and learn from defeat, athletes may benefit from noticing and savoring daily positive experiences, such as incremental sport improvement or time spent with teammates. Savoring is associated with increased positive emotions and healthy responses to daily negative events, suggesting savoring may be at least as effective as other frequently studied regulatory strategies focused solely on negative events/emotions (e.g., cognitive reappraisal, acceptance). To our knowledge, only two experience sampling studies of savoring exist (Jose et al. 2012; Sytine et al. 2019) and none have focused on athletes or compared the effects of savoring to other regulatory strategies on daily emotions. Using daily diary assessments over a two-week span during athletes’ competitive seasons, we tested the following hypotheses:

  1. 1.

    Controlling for negative event intensity, daily emotion regulation strategies will be associated with the quality of daily emotional experiences. Specifically, greater use of cognitive reappraisal, acceptance, problem-solving, and social support in response to daily negative events will be associated with more daily happiness, contentment, and gratitude and less sadness, anger, and annoyance. Greater use of cognitive and behavioral avoidance in response to daily negative events will be associated with the opposite (i.e., less daily happiness, contentment, and gratitude and more sadness, anger, and annoyance).

  2. 2.

    Consistent with existing theory (Bryant and Veroff 2007) and research (Jose et al. 2012), savoring daily positive events will moderate associations between positive event intensity and the quality of daily emotions such that greater savoring will strengthen the positive association between positive event intensity and positive emotions and strengthen the negative association between positive event intensity and negative emotions.

  3. 3.

    Similar to the most effective regulatory strategies from Hypothesis 1, savoring positive events will moderate (buffer) associations between greater daily negative event intensity and (1) less positive emotion and (2) more negative emotions at the daily level.

Method

Participants and Procedures

Participants were 67 collegiate athletes from George Mason University (GMU; n = 53) and Catholic University of America (CUA; n = 14). Due to the COVID-19 pandemic, collegiate sports were suspended during the Spring 2020 season. Thus, we were forced to un-enroll an additional 30 Spring sport athletes who signed consent. Our final sample of athletes represented various sports, including women’s soccer, women’s volleyball, men’s and women’s swimming and diving, men’s and women’s basketball, men’s and women’s cross country and track and field, women’s lacrosse, and softball. Recruitment procedures differed slightly between universities. At GMU, team coaches were contacted via email and given general study information. If coaches expressed interest, a member of the research staff scheduled a meeting with their teams to explain our study, train athletes on the daily diary software (PACO Personal Analytics Companion; Evans 2017), and obtain informed consent. At CUA, athletes were recruited directly via flyers and mass emails. Athletes were eligible to participate if they spoke and read English and owned a smartphone with a reliable internet connection. The average age of the final sample was 19.85 (SD = 1.25). Participants were 89% women; 91.1% White, 3.5% Hispanic/Latino, 2.4% Asian/Pacific Islander, and 2.9% Other.

Athletes who provided written consent to participate were re-contacted via email and invited to complete baseline questionnaires and demographics (as part of a larger study on college athlete resilience) followed by a daily diary assessment via PACO (Personal Analytics Companion; Evans 2017). Athletes from different teams completed the daily diary portion of the study at different times during their respective seasons, which were specified by coaches (at GMU) or the athletes themselves (at CUA) based on the number and importance of practices and competitions. Participants were pinged daily at 7:00 PM for 14 consecutive days to complete short, 5–10 min surveys about their day, which included questions about their most positive and negative events that day, positive and negative emotions, and regulatory responses to their most positive and negative events. Participants were instructed to complete surveys after finishing all sport-related activities and before 3:00 AM the following day. Participants were compensated with up to $40 in Amazon eGift cards for their participation ($10 for baseline assessment, up to $30 for completing all daily diaries). All procedures were approved by both universities’ IRBs.

Measures

Positive and Negative Emotions

Daily positive and negative emotions were measured using select emotion adjectives from the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule-Extended Form (PANAS-X; Watson and Clark 1999) using the stem, “How much does this word describe your mood today?” Responses were on a 5-point Likert scale from (1 = “Very slightly or not at all, 2 = “A little, 3 = “Moderately, 4 = “Quite a bit, 5 = “Extremely”). Original emotion items from the PANAS-X included, Cheerful, Joyful, Content, Sad, and Angry. We added two additional items for this study: Grateful and Annoyed. We used only seven emotion items for greater simplicity and lower participant burden while capturing emotions across the valance and arousal dimensions (e.g., Joyful = high valence/high arousal, Content = high valence/low arousal, Angry = low Valance/high arousal, Sad = low valence/low arousal; see Gerber et al. 2008). Since Joyful and Cheerful were highly correlated at the between- (r = 0.95) and within-person level (r = 0.70), we combined to form a composite variable, Happy. The resulting three positive emotion adjectives were averaged to create the positive emotions scale (RC = 0.84), and the three negative emotion adjectives were averaged to create the negative emotions scale (RC = 0.77). To explore the impact of regulatory strategies on specific positive and negative emotions, we entered individual emotion items/adjectives as outcomes in analyses for Hypotheses 2 and 3.

Positive and Negative Events

Participants reported on their most positive event that day with the following item: “Please describe today’s most positive event. Be as specific as you can. They rated the intensity of their most positive events (“How positive was this event?”) on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = “Not at all, 2 = “A little, 3 = “Moderately, 4 = “Quite a bit, 5 = “Extremely”). Participants also reported on their most negative event that day (“Please describe today’s most negative event. Be as specific as you can.”). Consistent with the primary and secondary appraisal model of coping (Lazarus 2006; Lazarus and Folkman 1984), participants then provided an appraisal of the intensity of their most negative event each day (“How negative was this event?”) using the same 5-point Likert scale.

Ways of Savoring Checklist (WOSC; Bryant and Veroff 2007)

Participants rated the extent they savored their most positive daily events using four WOSC items with the highest factor loadings from Jose et al. (2012). Items were rated on a 5-point Likert Scale (1 = “Not at all, 2 = “A little, 3 = “Moderately, 4 = “Quite a bit, 5 = “Extremely”). Items included, “I talked to another person about how good I felt,” “I looked for other people to share it with,” “I thought about what a lucky person I am that so many good things have happened to me,” and “I thought about sharing the memory of this later with other people.” Savoring items were averaged together to create a total savoring score (RC = 0.75).

Emotion Regulation Strategies

Participants rated the extent that they used various emotion regulation strategies in response to daily negative events using items from Aldridge-Gerry and colleagues’ daily coping scale. Items were rated on a 4-point Likert scale (1 = “Not at all, 2 = “A little, 3 = “A medium amount, 4 = “A lot”). The factor structure of this scale was validated in an undergraduate sample (Roesch et al. 2010). Items from this scale were drawn from other published coping measures with valid total scores in college student and older adolescent samples, including the Brief COPE (Carver 1997; Carver et al. 1989) and the Responses to Stress Questionnaire (Connor-Smith et al. 2000). For the present study, we focused on frequently researched regulatory strategies that we believed would be associated with SC, including, Social Support Seeking (RC = 0.75) – comprised of Emotion-Focused (two items; e.g., “I talked to my family about how I was feeling”) and Problem-Focused Support (RC = 0.82) (two items; e.g., “I figured out what I could do by talking to my friends”), Cognitive Reappraisal (RC = 0.78) (2 items; originally called “positive cognitive restructuring,” e.g., “I reminded myself that things could be worse”), Acceptance (RC = 0.32) (two items; e.g., “I learned to live with it”), Problem-Solving (RC = 0.82) – comprised of Direct Problem-Solving (2 items; e.g., “I did something to solve the problem”) and Cognitive Decision-Making (2 items; e.g., “I thought about what I need to know to solve the problem”), Behavioral Avoidance (RC = 0.51) (two items; originally called “avoidant actions,” e.g., “I tried to stay away from the problem”), and Cognitive Avoidance (RC = 0.43) (two items; e.g., I tried to put it out of my mind). Research suggests that this measure, and the scales from which it is adapted, predict daily alcohol consumption (Aldridge-Gerry et al. 2011), trait levels of fear (Ollendick et al. 2001), heart-rate reactivity to stress and internalizing/externalizing symptoms (Connor-Smith et al. 2000), and changes in the symptom severity of psychological disorders (e.g., Meyer 2001). Subscale reliabilities will be further discussed in the results section.

Data Analytic Strategy

To evaluate the interdependence of observations, we examined the intraclass correlations (ICCs) for each outcome (daily happiness, gratitude, contentment, sadness, anger, and annoyance). Results showed a substantial proportion of variance was attributable to differences between people (ICC range = 0.23–0.48; Table 1). As such, hypotheses were tested using two-level models with daily observations (level 1) nested within people (level 2), though no level 2 variables were used as predictors or outcomes in analyses. All predictors were within-person mean-centered so that scores represented deviations from each athlete’s mean during the 2-week daily assessment period. In addition to unstandardized coefficients (b), we reported standardized coefficients (β) as a measure of effect size, which is a recommended approach in multi-level modeling (e.g. Lorah 2018).

Table 1 Between- and within-person correlations and descriptive statistics

The reliability of daily multi-item scales was calculated in SPSS based on G Theory (e.g. Brennan 1992; Shrout and Lane 2012) using code specified by Bolger and Laurenceau (2013). This approach is optimal for repeated daily measures in multi-level models and allowed us to account for multiple sources of variance, including differences between people, items, and time (i.e., days). Specifically, our index of reliability (“RC) assessed the extent to which within-person changes were reliable across days. RC is higher when variance is predominantly attributable to differences across people and time rather than differences across items and error.

Primary analyses were performed using the lme4 package (Bates et al. 2007) in R 3.6.1 (R Core Team, 2019). In order to reduce limitations related to our smaller sample size at the between-person level, our analyses focused exclusively on within-person predictions to harness all 836 daily diary responses across participants. To test the effects of daily emotion regulation strategies (in response to negative events) on daily emotions (Hypothesis 1), we entered each strategy – cognitive reappraisal, acceptance, problem-solving, social support seeking, cognitive avoidance, and behavioral avoidance – as predictors of daily positive and negative emotions in separate models. Next, we included significant predictors from these models together to test which strategies predicted unique variance in daily positive and negative emotions, again, controlling for negative event intensity. Since the intensity of daily negative events was correlated with daily positive (r = -0.31) and negative emotions (r = 0.45) at the within-person level (Table 1), we entered negative event intensity as a covariate in all models for Hypothesis 1.

To test the effects of positive event intensity, savoring positive events, and their interaction on daily emotions (Hypothesis 2), we entered these variables as predictors of daily positive and negative emotions. Since the intensity of daily positive events was correlated with positive (r = 0.33) and negative emotions (r = − 0.16) at the within-person level, we entered positive event intensity as a covariate in all models for Hypothesis 2. To test the emotionally protective effects of emotion regulation strategies related to both negative and positive events (Hypothesis 3), we selected the regulatory strategies that predicted (more) positive and/or (less) negative daily emotions from Hypothesis 1. In one set of models, we entered negative event intensity as a predictor of daily emotions moderated by the most effective regulatory strategies from Hypothesis 1. In another set of models, we entered negative event intensity as a predictor of daily emotions moderated by the extent participants savored their most positive events (with positive event intensity as a covariate). For Hypotheses 2 and 3, we entered individual positive (happy, grateful, content) and negative emotions (sad, angry, annoyed) along with average levels of each as outcomes.

Results

Descriptive Statistics

Between- and within-person correlations and descriptive statistics for primary study variables are presented in Table 1. Participants who completed fewer than 7 daily diaries in total were excluded from analyses (N = 8). The remaining participants (N = 67) completed an average of 12.89 daily diaries (SD = 2.19) for a total of 836 daily observations. Due to forced response settings on the PACO app, completed daily diaries had no missing data. Participants were instructed to delete the PACO app and discontinue their completion of daily diaries after 14 days. However, several participants completed more than the 14 required daily diaries. In these cases, up to two diary entries past the 14th day were accepted, and any additional diary entries were removed from analyses. Daily diary compliance was not significantly correlated with daily positive and negative emotions at the within-person level and was thus not accounted for during analyses.

The reliability of primary daily measures was generally high except for the two-item acceptance scale (RC = 0.32). When examining the items comprising this scale, it is understandable that participants may have responded differently to, “I learned to live with it” versus, “I just accepted the fact that this is the way it is.” While this scale was designed for daily use (e.g. Aldridge-Gerry et al. 2011), “learning to live with it” may be less applicable to daily stressors (which are often transient and do not require long-term recalibration of expectations) and thus, may have decreased internal consistency. Still, our acceptance measure arguably captures two different features of acceptance, broadening content validity compared to a single-item measure. It is no surprise that reliability was lower on average for 2-item daily scales (except for cognitive reappraisal; RC = 0.78) compared to measures with three or four items (e.g. positive and negative emotions, savoring). As a caveat, appropriate methods for calculating the reliability of daily measures in multilevel models are poorly understood, and when done correctly, reliability may be lower than when using conventional methods (e.g., cronbach’s alpha) as if observations were independent (i.e. not nested) (Nezlek 2011, 2012).

Hypothesis Testing

Hypothesis 1:

Daily regulatory strategies related to negative events predicting daily emotions.

Multilevel regression results (Table 2) revealed that, controlling for negative event intensity, greater use of daily problem solving predicted more daily positive emotions. Greater use of daily cognitive reappraisal and acceptance were associated with more positive and less negative emotions, also controlling for negative event intensity. Contrary to hypotheses, greater use of daily social support was not associated with daily positive emotions and was associated with more negative emotions. As hypothesized, greater use of cognitive avoidance and behavioral avoidance were associated with more daily negative emotions but were not associated with positive emotions. When combining regulatory strategies that predicted more positive and less negative daily emotions in the same models (Table 3), cognitive reappraisal was the only regulatory strategy that predicted more daily positive emotions while acceptance was the only regulatory strategy that predicted less daily negative emotions (controlling for negative event intensity in both models).

Table 2 Multilevel regression results with regulatory strategies predicting same day emotions, controlling for the intensity of negativity events
Table 3 Multilevel regressions testing unique variance explained in daily positive and negative emotions by significant predictors from Table 2, controlling for negative event intensity

Hypothesis 2:

Positive event intensity predicting daily emotions, moderated by savoring.

Positive event intensity predicted more positive and less negative emotions. Daily savoring predicted more positive emotions and less sadness, annoyance, and average negative emotions controlling for positive event intensity. There were also significant interaction effects between daily positive event intensity and savoring predicting daily happiness, gratitude, contentment, such that greater positive event intensity was more strongly associated with positive emotions when athletes savored these events more (Table 4, Fig. 1).

Table 4 Multilevel regression models with positive event intensity predicting daily emotions, moderated by savoring of positive events
Fig. 1
figure 1

Interactions between the intensity of positive daily events and savoring predicting daily positive emotions

Hypothesis 3:

Negative event intensity predicting daily emotions, moderated by cognitive reappraisal, acceptance, and savoring

In models containing cognitive reappraisal, there were main effects for negative event intensity predicting less positive emotion and cognitive reappraisal predicting more positive emotions. Negative event intensity and cognitive reappraisal interacted to predict gratitude such that greater daily cognitive reappraisal buffered the negative association between negative event intensity and gratitude. The negative event intensity x cognitive reappraisal interaction did not predict any other positive or negative emotions (Table 5, Fig. 2).

Table 5 Multilevel regression models comparing cognitive reappraisal and savoring positive events as moderators of the association between negative event intensity and daily positive emotions
Fig. 2
figure 2

Interaction between the intensity of daily negative events and reappraising negative events predicting daily gratitude

In models containing acceptance, there were main effects for negative event intensity and acceptance predicting less negative emotion. Negative event intensity and acceptance interacted to predict less sadness, anger, and average negative emotions (but not annoyance) such that greater daily acceptance buffered positive associations between negative event intensity and sadness, anger, and average negative emotions (Table 6, Fig. 3).

Table 6 Multilevel regression models comparing acceptance and savoring positive events as moderators of the association between negative event intensity and daily negative emotions
Fig. 3
figure 3

Interactions between the intensity of daily negative events and accepting negative events predicting negative emotions

In models containing savoring, there were main effects for positive event intensity predicting more negative and less positive emotion, negative event intensity predicting less positive and greater negative emotion, and savoring predicting more positive emotion but not less negative emotion. Negative event intensity and savoring interacted to predict daily anger, annoyance, and average negative emotions (but not sadness) such that greater savoring buffered positive associations between negative event intensity and anger, annoyance, and average negative emotions. Negative event intensity and savoring did not interact to predict positive emotions (Table 6, Fig. 4).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Interactions between the intensity of daily negative events and savoring positive events predicting daily negative emotions, controlling for the intensity of positive events being savored

Discussion

Using a two-week experience sampling approach, we found that, controlling for the intensity of negative events, greater daily use of cognitive reappraisal and acceptance in response to these events predicted more positive and less negative emotion while problem solving predicted only more positive emotion. Cognitive avoidance, behavioral avoidance, and interestingly, social support seeking each predicted more daily negative emotion and did not predict positive emotions after controlling for the intensity of negative events.

When combining effective emotion regulation strategies into the same models (i.e. cognitive reappraisal, acceptance, and problem solving), only cognitive reappraisal and acceptance predicted unique variance in (more) positive and (less) negative daily emotion, respectively. Controlling for positive event intensity, savoring predicted more positive emotions and less sadness and annoyance but not anger. Savoring also moderated (strengthened) the association between positive event intensity and positive emotions. Finally, when comparing the moderating effects of cognitive reappraisal, acceptance, and savoring on the association between daily negative events and emotions, accepting negative events and savoring positive events provided a greater buffer against negative emotional outcomes compared to cognitive reappraisal, which only buffered against the negative association between daily negative event intensity and gratitude.

The fact that cognitive reappraisal and acceptance emerged as significant, unique predictors of positive and negative emotions is consistent with a number of experience sampling and laboratory studies with non-athlete populations (Dunn et al. 2009; Eifert and Heffner 2003; Jamieson et al. 2013; Nezlek and Kuppens 2008; Troy et al. 2010) as well as cognitive-behavioral and mindfulness-based clinical interventions. These regulatory strategies are explored less frequently among college athletes in favor of studies focused on sport-specific coping and the effects of regulatory strategies on athletic performance (e.g. Hanin 2007; Jones 2012; Laborde et al. 2014; Lane et al. 2011). Adopting a broader view, our study demonstrates the effectiveness of various emotion regulation strategies related to both positive and negative events in daily life, which could be sport or non-sport related. As awareness of and responsivity to athlete mental health concerns has finally begun to increase, it is important to understand emotion regulation within and beyond the sport context. Of course, stressful events from outside sport can impair sport performance without effective emotion regulation (e.g. Cosh and Tully 2015).

The fact that social support-seeking predicted more daily negative emotions may seem initially peculiar. Social support is often considered an adaptive form of emotion regulation, which promotes emotional and physical health (e.g. Demaray and Malecki 2002; Frasure-Smith et al. 2000; Turner 1981). However, some research suggests that social support-seeking in the form of co-rumination (i.e. discussing and revisiting problems, speculating about their cause, and focusing on negative feelings), is common among close friend groups (e.g. athletic teams) and associated with elevated depression and anxiety (Rose et al. 2007). It may be that social support-seeking took the form of co-rumination in our sample of young, predominantly female athletes and was thus associated with poor emotional outcomes.

It is important to differentiate social support seeking from the perception that one is obtaining the support they desire. Our measurement approach captured the act of seeking emotional and/or practical support from friends or family, not the perception of whether support was received or adequate. It may be that seeking social support was associated with increased negative emotions because athletes sought but never obtained adequate support. Research and theory suggest that seeking but not receiving social support is associated with a host of negative outcomes whether social support was sought in person (e.g. Cohen and Willis 1985) or via social media (Frison and Eggermont 2015). In fact, thwarted attempts at obtaining adequate support, along with perceiving that one is a burden on others (which may stem from these thwarted attempts), are well-established predictors of suicidal ideation among at-risk populations, including LGBTQ college students (e.g. Hill and Pettit 2012; Hill et al. 2017). Research should further explore how college athletes seek social support, particularly from their teammates and coaches, and which strategies for seeking and giving social support promote healthy responses to distress.

To our knowledge, our study is the first to explore savoring in the daily lives of athletes. When coaches, sport psychology consultants, and researchers focus solely on regulating negative emotions in response to negative events, another dimension of daily emotion and experience is ignored. Promising findings continue to emerge in the field of positive psychology, showing that interventions enhancing character strengths, gratitude, savoring, and compassion not only enhance positive emotions, but facilitate healthy responses to distress (Chaves et al. 2017; McMakin et al. 2011; Meyer et al. 2012; Sin and Lyubomirsky 2009; Smith and Hanni 2019). Our results add to this literature, suggesting that savoring, while unrelated to negative events, may be effective in regulating emotional responses to such events.

Mindfulness is widely considered an important trait for athlete performance and well-being (e.g. Aherne et al. 2011; Haase et al. 2015; Kaufman et al. 2009), but despite convincing evidence, savoring is not typically considered under the umbrella of mindfulness in athletic contexts. For example, a recent study suggests that team-based interventions with athletes that include gratitude and savoring components are effective in reducing sport burnout and enhancing sport satisfaction and well-being (Gabana et al. 2019). Research should explore whether more mindful athletes engage in more savoring, as they are adept at shifting their attention and encoding positive stimuli more deeply. Athletes who are less mindful may be less accepting of distress, become more entangled with it, and make greater attempts to change it (e.g. via cognitive reappraisal or other strategies).

The fact that savoring enhances athletes’ emotional experience is not particularly surprising given findings from existing research with the general population (Bryant and Veroff 2007; Jose et al. 2012; Silton et al. 2020; Sytine et al. 2019; Quoidbach et al. 2015). Although, it is notable that savoring positive events buffered the effects of daily negative events on daily emotions – similar to accepting these negative events and more so than reappraising them. Cognitive reappraisal is often touted as an optimal regulatory strategy (e.g. Gross and John 2003; Haga et al. 2009), but many studies fail to consider the contexts and individual differences that modulate its effectiveness. Recent research suggests that many individuals have difficulty using reappraisal effectively and that reappraisal can be ineffective in certain situations, such as when taking deliberate action would be more effective or when negative emotions are useful for achieving goals (e.g. Ford and Troy 2019; Troy et al. 2013).

Our findings add to this literature and other work on the benefits of acceptance and cognitive reappraisal (e.g. Vilardaga et al. 2013). Future research should not only explore reappraisal and acceptance among athletes individually, but also their co-occurrence. Emerging research suggests that some individuals may use multiple emotion regulation strategies at the same time, such as reappraisal and emotional suppression, with greater benefits than using only one (Sahdra et al. 2020). It would be valuable to know whether a certain combination of regulatory strategies used together enhance emotional outcomes for some athletes but not others.

Limitations and Future Directions

Our findings contribute new information to the study of emotion regulation among athletes, but several limitations must be considered. First, our analyses were correlational. While separate studies suggest reappraisal, acceptance, and savoring to play a causal role in reducing emotional distress, future experimental studies should compare the reappraisal, acceptance, and savoring interventions on emotional outcomes. Second, our findings are contingent upon our measurement approach. We used the four savoring items with the highest factor loadings from a widely used scale (Bryant and Veroff 2007), and these items predominantly captured the social aspects of savoring (e.g. “I talked to another person about how good I felt,”) along with counting blessings (“I thought about what a lucky person I am that so many good things have happened to me”). Future studies should assess a wider range of savoring strategies at the daily level.

The generalizability of our findings is also limited by the scope of our daily cognitive reappraisal measure. We used two items from a daily coping scale published by Aldridge-Gerry et al. 2011, which captured “positive” cognitive reappraisal, specifically (“I reminded myself that things could be worse,” “I tried to think about or notice only the good things in life”) rather than general cognitive reappraisal (e.g. changing one’s thinking about a situation to be more rational). The nature of our reappraisal measure may explain high between-person correlations with daily savoring (r = 0.71) since both measures capture a tendency to interpret events more positively. However, within-person correlations capturing the covariation of savoring and reappraisal each day across people were low (0.11). Still, while the cognitive behavioral therapy tradition emphasizes rational thinking, athletes and other populations may practice reappraisal differently, perhaps valuing positive over rational thinking. Using a succinct measure of daily emotion regulation was crucial for minimizing participant burnout, as we wished to measure a wide range of strategies each day. Future studies focused on reappraisal or a smaller range of strategies would benefit from adopting measures with more items and stronger content validity.

Third, with 67 athletes, our ability to conduct between-person analyses yielding stable results was limited. To harness all 836 daily observations, our hypotheses focused exclusively on within-person analyses. Despite barriers to recruiting college athletes for intensive longitudinal studies, researchers should strive to obtain larger samples to examine individual differences in within-person predictors of emotion regulation, including conscientiousness, negative emotionality, mindfulness, and self-compassion. It may be that individuals with higher trait levels of self-compassion are more likely to savor positive experiences due to beliefs that they are worthy. Given links between self-compassion and mindfulness, individuals with greater self-compassion may also derive greater benefits from savoring due to a tendency to live in the present.

Fourth, participants in our sample overwhelmingly identified as white (91.1%). Experience sampling data suggest that individuals from racial, ethnic, and sexual minority groups use avoidant coping strategies more frequently on days when stigma-related stressors are reported (Hatzenbeuhler et al. 2009). Replications with more diverse samples may yield a wider range of reported positive and negative daily events and differences in terms of use of regulatory strategies and their impact on daily emotions.

Lastly, while we approached a range of men’s and women’s athletic teams, our final sample was overwhelmingly female (89%). It is possible that the sex make-up of our sample significantly influenced our results and the generalizability of these results to the full spectrum of college athletes. Data suggest that women tend to use a wider range of emotion regulation strategies compared to men (Nolen-Hoeksema and Aldao 2011; Thoits 1991). Women may also down-regulate negative emotions by upregulating positive emotions to a greater extent than men (e.g. McRae et al. 2008). Regarding savoring specifically, some evidence suggests that women may be more inclined to savor positive experiences than men (e.g. Bryant 2003; Kim and Bryant 2017) and are more open to positive psychological interventions to enhance savoring (e.g. Thompson et al. 2015). Taken together, this suggests that our findings related to savoring might not generalize to male athletes, but data are needed to support this claim. Researchers might consider exploring differences in savoring use and effectiveness among men. Researchers can also explore barriers and facilitators to implementing positive psychological interventions focused on savoring with men in athletic contexts specifically.

Conclusions

Despite these limitations, our study contributes valuable information to research on athlete emotion regulation and well-being. Our findings suggest that savoring positive events may be equally beneficial to accepting negative ones, and superior to cognitive reappraisal, in buffering against the negative emotional consequences of daily negative events. Coaches at the collegiate level and beyond often reference the importance of savoring wins, then immediately getting back to work. There is less emphasis on deliberately paying attention to and noticing positive emotions about productive practices, smaller individual improvements, downtime, and positive experiences with teammates. Our results underscore the potential importance of savoring daily positive events for mitigating emotional distress during the competitive season.

Our findings also have implications for the design of well-being and resilience focused interventions. While traditional clinical interventions, such as cognitive behavioral therapy, tend to focus on alleviating negative thoughts and emotions (e.g. with cognitive reappraisal), there has been a surge of interventions which aim instead to bolster psychological strengths and well-being (e.g. “Positive Psychotherapy;” Seligman et al. 2006). Positive psychotherapeutic interventions are primarily designed to upregulate positive states, traits, and experiences, but they may also reduce negative states and alleviate symptoms of serious mental health disorders, such as major depression and schizophrenia (e.g. Chaves et al. 2017; Meyer et al. 2012; Sin and Lyubomirsky 2009). Savoring-focused interventions also show promise for promoting healthy responses to stress and reducing depressed mood (Ho et al. 2014; Hurley and Kwon, 2012; Meyer et al. 2012; Smith and Hanni 2019). We hope this research program increases attention toward savoring as a way of managing the various stressors inherent to college athletics.