Skip to main content
Log in

Eye movements are primed toward the center of multiple stimuli even when the interstimulus distances are too large to generate saccade averaging

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Prior oculomotor research has established that saccades tend to land near the center of multiple saccade targets when they are near each other. This saccade averaging phenomenon (or global effect) has been ascribed to short-distance lateral excitation between neurons in the superior colliculus. Further, at greater inter-stimulus distances, eye movements tend toward the individual elements. This transition to control by local elements (individuation) with inter-stimulus distance has been attributed to long-range lateral inhibition between neurons in winner-take-all models of oculomotor behavior. We hypothesized that the traditional method of requiring a saccade to an array of multiple, simultaneous targets may entail response ambiguity that intensifies with distance. We resolved the ambiguity by focussing on reaction time of our human participants to a single saccade target after one or more simultaneous priming stimuli. At a 50-ms prime-target interval, saccadic reaction time was shortest for targets closer to the center of the prime stimuli independent of the distance between the primes. This effect was gone at 400 ms. These findings challenge the typical inferences about the neural control of oculomotor behavior that have been derived from the boundary between saccade averaging and individuation and provide a new method to explore eye movements with lessened impact from decision processes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Results from trials with single primes and nearby (45° away) targets allowed us to confirm the null results reported with single targets from the studies by Wang et al. (2012a, b) and Watanabe (2001). Saccade landing positions were measured with respect to how much they deviated toward or away from that primeQuery. At the 50 ms PTOA the mean of deviation was 0.17° (away from the cued side), 95 % CI = [−0.09°, 0.42°]. At the 400 ms PTOA the mean deviation was −0.3° (toward the cued side), 95 % CI = [−0.6°, 0.01°]. Thus, in agreement with Wang et al. and Watanabe, with confidence intervals that include zero, there is no compelling evidence here to suggest that single primes bias saccade landing sites when single targets are presented at unknown locations.

  2. Further examination of errors in landing position revealed that the median Euclidean distance between landing position and the center of the target for the correct trials was approximately 1°. There was also no relationship between accuracy of landing position and SRT (r = −0.03).

References

  • Akaike H (1974) A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Trans Autom Control 19:716–723

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arai K, Keller EL (2005) A model of the saccade-generating system that accounts for trajectory variations produced by competing visual stimuli. Biol Cybern 92:21–37

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S (2014) lme4: linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.1-7. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4

  • Box GEP, Cox DR (1964) An analysis of transformations. J R Stat Soc Ser B 26(2):211–252

    Google Scholar 

  • Chou I, Sommer MA, Schiller PH (1999) Express averaging saccades in monkeys. Vis Res 39:4200–4216

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Christie J, Hilchey MD, Klein RM (2014) Inhibition of return is at the midpoint of simultaneous cues. Atten Percept Psychophys 75(8):1610–1618. doi:10.3758/s13414-013-0510-5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coren S, Hoenig P (1972) Effect of non-target stimuli upon length of voluntary saccades. Percept Mot Skills 34:499–508

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dorris MC, Olivier E, Munoz D (2007) Competitive integration of visual and preparatory signals in the superior colliculus during saccadic programming. J Neurosci 27:5053–5062

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edelman JA, Keller EL (1998) Dependence on target configuration of express saccade-related activity in the primate superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 80:1407–1426

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Findlay JM (1982) Global visual processing for saccadic eye movements. Vis Res 22:1033–1045

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Findlay JM (1997) Saccade target selection in visual search. Vis Res 37:617–631

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Findlay JM, Brown V (2006) Eye scanning of multi-element displays: II. Saccade planning. Vis Res 46:216–227

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gabay S, Leibovich T, Ben-Simon A, Henik A, Segev R (2012) Inhibition of return in the archer fish. Nat Commun 4(1657):1–5

    Google Scholar 

  • Glimcher PW, Sparks DL (1993) Representation of averaging saccades in the superior colliculus of the monkey. Exp Brain Res 95:429–435

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Godijn R, Theeuwes J (2002) Oculomotor capture and inhibition of return: evidence for an oculomotor suppression account of IOR. Psychol Res 66(4):234–246

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hilchey MD, Klein RM, Satel J (2014) Returning to “inhibition of return” by dissociating long-term oculomotor IOR from short-term sensory adaptation and other nonoculomotor “inhibitory” cueing effects. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 40(4):1603–1616

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Isa T, Hall WC (2009) Exploring the superior colliculus in vitro. J Neurophysiol 102:2581–2593

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klein RM (2000) Inhibition of return. Trends Cogn Sci 4:138–147

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klein RM, Christie J, Morris EP (2005) Vector averaging of inhibition of return. Psychon Bull Rev 12(2):295–300

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kowler E (1990) The role of visual and cognitive processes in the control of eye movement. In: Kowler E (ed) Eye movements and their role in visual and cognitive processes. Elsevier, Amsterdam

    Google Scholar 

  • Langley LK, Gayzur ND, Saville AL, Morlock SL, Bagne AG (2011) Spatial distribution of attentional inhibition is not altered in healthy aging. Atten Percept Psychophys 73:766–783

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee P, Hall W (2006) An in vitro study of horizontal connections in the intermediate layer of the superior colliculus. J Neurosci 26:4763–4768

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marino RA, Trappenberg TP, Dorris M, Munoz DP (2011) Spatial interactions in the superior colliculus predict saccade behavior in a neural field model. J Cogn Neurosci 24(2):315–336

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacPherson AC, Klein RM, Moore C (2003) Inhibition of return in children and adolescents. J Exp Child Psychol 85:337–351. doi:10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00104-8

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meeter M, Van der Stigchel S, Theeuwes J (2010) A competitive integration model of exogenous and endogenous eye movements. Biol Cybern 102(4):271–291

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer D, Schwaneveldt R (1971) Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: evidence of dependence between retrieval operations. J Exp Psychol 90:227–234

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neely JH (1976) Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: evidence for facilitatory and inhibitory processes. Memory Congn 4(5):648–654

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Neely JH (1977) Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. J Exp Psychol Gen 106(3):226

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ottes FP, Van Gisbergen JAM, Eggermont JJ (1984) Metrics of saccade responses to visual double stimuli: two different modes. Vis Res 24(10):1169–1179

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner MI, Snyder CRR (1975) Attention and cognitive control. In: Solso RL (ed) Information processing and cognition: the loyola symposium. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner MI, Rafal RD, Choate LS, Vaughan J (1985) Inhibition of return: neural basis and function. Cogn Neuropsychol 2:211–228

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pratt J, Fischer MH (2002) Examining the role of the fixation cue in inhibition of return. Canadian J Exp Psychol/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale 56:294–301. doi:10.1037/h0087405

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • R Development Core Team (2011) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. ISBN 3-900051-07-0, http://www.R-project.org/

  • Robinson DA (1972) Eye movements evoked by collicular stimulation in the alert monkey. Vis Res 12:1795–1808

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sapir A, Soroker N, Berger A, Henik A (1999) Inhibition of return in spatial attention: direct evidence for collicular generation. Nat Neurosci 2(12):1053–1054

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Satel J, Wang Z, Trappenberg TP, Klein RM (2011) Modeling inhibition of return as short-term depression of early sensory input to the superior colliculus. Vis Res 51:987–996

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schiller PH, True SD, Conway JL (1979) Paired stimulation of the frontal eye fields and the superior colliculus of the rhesus monkey. Brain Res 179:162–164

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sereno AB, Briand KA, Amador SC, Szapiel SV (2006) Disruption of reflexive attention and eye movements in an individual with a collicular lesion. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 28:145–166

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Symonds MRR, Moussalli A (2011) A brief guide to model selection, multimodel inference and model averaging in behavioral ecology using Akaike’s information criterion. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 65:13–21

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Stigchel S, Nijboer TCW (2013) How global is the global effect? The spatial characteristics of saccade averaging. Vis Res 84:6–15

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Stigchel S, de Vries JP, Bethlehem R, Theeuwes J (2011) A global effect of capture saccades. Exp Brain Res 210:57–65

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Stigchel S, Heeman J, Nijboer TCW (2012) Averaging is not everything: the saccade global effect weakens with increasing stimulus size. Vis Res 62:108–115

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Opstal AJ, Van Gisbergen JAM (1989) A nonlinear model for collicular spatial interactions underlying the metrical properties of electrically elicited saccades. Biol Cybern 60:171–183

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Opstal AJ, Van Gisbergen JAM (1990) Role of monkey superior colliculus in saccade averaging. Exp Brain Res 79:143–149

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vitu F (2008) About the global effect and the critical role of retinal eccentricity: implications for eye movements in reading. J Eye Mov Res 2:1–18

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker R, Deubel H, Schneider WX, Findlay JM (1997) Effect of remote distractors on saccade programming: evidence for an extended fixation zone. J Neurophysiol 78:1108–1119

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang Z, Kruijne W, Theeuwes J (2012a) Lateral interactions in the superior colliculus produce saccade deviation in a neural field model. Vis Res 62:66–74

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang Z, Satel J, Hilchey MD, Klein RM (2012b) Averaging saccades are repelled by prior uninformative cues at both short and long intervals. Vis Cogn 20:825–847

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watanabe K (2001) Inhibition of return of averaging saccades. Exp Brain Res 138:330–342. doi:10.1007/s002210100709

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilimzig C, Schneider S, Schoener G (2006) The time course of saccadic decision making: dynamic field theory. Neural Netw 19(8):1059–1074

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was funded by an NSERC grant to Raymond M. Klein and was made possible by a Killam Fellowship to Matthew Hilchey.

Conflict of interest

None.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John Christie.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Christie, J., Hilchey, M.D., Mishra, R. et al. Eye movements are primed toward the center of multiple stimuli even when the interstimulus distances are too large to generate saccade averaging. Exp Brain Res 233, 1541–1549 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4227-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4227-7

Keywords

Navigation