Introduction

figure a

Dementia is one of the most common neurodegenerative diseases worldwide [1] and results in poor quality of life among the impacted individuals and a serious public health burden for society. Cognitive decline occurs over a long period prior to dementia and is important for the monitoring and early intervention of cognitive deterioration [2]. Thus, the identification of risk factors for cognitive decline could contribute to screening individuals who are at risk of dementia.

Long-term variability in HbA1c has been proposed as an important risk factor related to mortality [3,4,5,6], cardiovascular disease (CVD) [7,8,9], nephropathy [10,11,12] and other complications [13,14,15,16,17] independent of HbA1c levels. Recently, there has been considerable interest in the emerging association between glycaemic variability and decline in cognitive function [18,19,20,21,22]. Although the previous studies mainly focused on individuals with type 2 diabetes, evidence from the general population (especially people without diabetes) is still scarce.

The Health and Retirement Study (HRS) and the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA) were two sister cohorts that included a large and diverse population (age ≥ 50 years), and in these studies, repeated measurements of HbA1c and cognitive assessments at set time intervals were performed. In this study, we aimed to identify the relationship between visit-to-visit variability (VVV) in HbA1c and the rate of cognitive decline in two elderly populations with normal cognition at baseline. We hypothesised that a higher variability in HbA1c would be associated with accelerated cognitive decline in the elderly population.

Methods

Study population

In this study, we used data from Wave 2006 to Wave 2016 of the HRS and Wave 2 (2002–2003) to Wave 8 (2014–2015) of the ELSA, two prospective and nationally representative cohorts conducted in the USA and England. Detailed descriptions of the objective, design and methods of these two cohorts have been described elsewhere [23, 24]. The time of the first HbA1c measurement was considered as the baseline for all participants. A flow chart of participant selection for the present study is shown in Fig. 1. In the HRS, a total of 3314 participants had three measurements of HbA1c from Wave 2006 to Wave 2016. Of these, 17 participants were excluded owing to lack of cognitive measurement at baseline, and 43 participants were excluded because of a history of dementia and/or Alzheimer’s disease at baseline. In the ELSA, a total of 2993 participants had three or four measurements of HbA1c from Wave 2 to Wave 8. Of these participants, ten were excluded due to lack of cognitive assessment at baseline (n = 5) or a history of dementia or Alzheimer’s disease at baseline (n = 5). The remaining participants from the HRS (n = 3254) and ELSA (n = 2983) with complete HbA1c measurements, baseline cognitive assessments and at least one reassessment of cognitive function were included in the analyses.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow chart of inclusion and exclusion of the participants in the current study

This study conformed to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki. Institutional Review Board approval of the HRS was obtained through the University of Michigan and the National Institute on Aging. The ELSA was approved by the London Multicentre Research Ethics Committee. Informed consent was obtained from all participants in both cohorts.

Measurement of HbA1c and HbA1c variability

Blood sample collection and measurement of HbA1c in the HRS and ELSA were conducted every 4 years. Details of the process have been described elsewhere [25, 26]. National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES)-equivalent assay values of HbA1c in the HRS were used in our study as recommended [26]. In this study, HbA1c was reported as the percentage of the erythrocyte haemoglobin that was glycated.

The mean HbA1c value was calculated based on the mean of all visits for each participant. The VVV in HbA1c was primarily defined as the intra-individual CV across visits. Due to the lack of an appropriate gold-standard measurement for HbA1c VVV, we calculated two other metrics, including the SD and the variation independent of the mean (VIM). These calculation processes have been previously described [27].

Measurement of cognitive function

Cognitive function was assessed every 2 years in both the HRS and ELSA using a variety of tests, including self-rated memory, immediate and delayed word recall, and backward count. In this study, we used scores of memory test and executive function as outcomes, which were assessed in both the HRS and ELSA. We created a total memory recall score ranging from 0 to 20 by summing the scores of immediate and delayed recall tests. A higher score on the memory recall test indicated better memory performance. Executive function was assessed by a verbal fluency task in which participants were required to orally list as many animal names as they could in 60 s. The score of verbal fluency was calculated as the total count of words excluding repeated and non-animal words, and higher scores indicated better executive function. Both immediate and delayed word recall tests and verbal fluency tests have been shown to have good validity and consistency [28, 29].

The standardised z score for the cognitive test scores at each wave was calculated by subtracting the mean score at baseline and dividing the value by the SD of the baseline scores. Thus, a z score of 1 means the performance on the particular cognitive test was 1 SD above the mean score at baseline.

Covariates

The covariates included demographic and clinical variables. The demographic variables included age, sex, educational level (college or above), BMI, living arrangement (living alone or not), current cigarette smoking, and current alcohol consumption (alcohol consumption at least 1 day per week). Clinical variables included history or presence of CVD (myocardial infarction, CHD, revascularisation, stroke and peripheral arterial disease of heart failure), hypertension, diabetes, lung disease and cancer. Depressive symptoms were measured using an 8-item version of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D). Mean systolic BP across visits was also calculated for each participant. Baseline HDL, tested in both the HRS and ELSA, was also included.

Statistical analysis

All participants were categorised into quartiles of CV of HbA1c. Categorical variables are presented as numbers (proportions), and continuous variables are presented as the mean ± SD. We used the Cochran–Armitage trend test for categorical variables and linear regression for continuous variables to test the significance of the trends across the quartiles.

We used a multivariable linear mixed-effect model (a widely used model to address repeated measurement data) to evaluate the longitudinal association between VVV in HbA1c and cognitive decline. In the current study, the intercept and slope were both fitted as random effects to address the inter-individual differences at baseline and different rates of cognitive function change during the follow-up period. A negative β value for the interaction item of time and CV of HbA1c indicated that a 1-unit increment of CV of HbA1c was associated with a faster rate of decline with increasing time in the study. Two models were used as follows: Model I included the CV of HbA1c (the lowest quartile as the reference group), time (years from baseline), interaction item of time and CV of HbA1c, mean HbA1c value, interaction item of time and mean HbA1c value, age (continuous variable) and sex (male or female). Model II included all covariates in Model I and was additionally adjusted for education, current smoking status (yes or no), alcohol consumption status (yes or no), BMI (continuous variable), hypertension (yes or no), diabetes (yes or no) and HDL (continuous variable). The p value for the trend was calculated by using the median value in each quartile of the HbA1c CV. The effect of each SD increment in HbA1c CV was also calculated by modelling HbA1c CV as a continuous variable. Subgroup analysis was conducted by stratifying the participants into two groups based on their status of diabetes at baseline: diabetes and non-diabetes. The effects were calculated within each cohort separately and were pooled using a random-effects model. Heterogeneity of β values between the two cohorts was evaluated by the Cochran’s Q test and I2 statistic.

The multiple imputation of chained equations (MICEs) method was also used to impute missing data from the cognitive assessments during follow-up. Baseline characteristics, including age, sex, education, BMI, smoking status, alcohol consumption status, diabetes, hypertension and baseline cognitive scores, were used to impute the missing values. We created 20 imputed datasets and pooled the results using the R package ‘MICE’ for each model. The imputation quality was assessed by comparing the imputed data with the original data using density plots. Sensitivity analyses were conducted as follows: (1) modelling SD or VIM instead of CV as VVV metrics of HbA1c; (2) repeating analysis with all available data without multiple imputations; (3) removing the participants with three HbA1c measurements in the ELSA from the analysis.

All statistical analyses were performed using R software 3.4.1 (R Foundation, Vienna, Austria). The R package ‘lme4’ was used to perform the linear mixed-effect model [30]. Two-sided p values less than 0.05 were considered statistically significant.

Results

Baseline characteristics

A total of 6237 participants (3254 from the HRS and 2983 from the ELSA) were included in the analysis. The mean age at baseline was 64.55 ± 9.11 years in the HRS and 62.03 ± 7.79 years in the ELSA. A total of 1974 (60.66%) participants in the HRS and 1658 (55.58%) participants in the ELSA were female. The mean follow-up times were 10.48 ± 0.63 years and 10.56 ± 1.86 years for the HRS and ELSA, respectively. A comparison of baseline characteristics between participants included and not included is shown in electronic supplementary material (ESM) Table 1.

Table 1 presents the characteristics of the included participants across quartiles of HbA1c CV. Participants in the highest quartile of HbA1c CV had a higher BMI, baseline HbA1c, mean HbA1c across visits, mean systolic BP, prevalence of hypertension and diabetes, and a lower education level and HDL in both the HRS and ELSA. Participants tended to have a lower baseline cognitive function (both total recall score and verbal fluency score) as the quartiles of HbA1c CV increased (p for trend <0.001).

Table 1 Baseline characteristics of the HRS and ELSA

Association of HbA1c variability with cognitive decline in the overall sample

As listed in Tables 2 and 3, a 1% increase in the mean HbA1c value was associated with an increased rate of decline in the memory z score (−0.089 SD/year, 95% CI −0.165, −0.014) and executive function z score (−0.058 SD/year, 95% CI −0.111, −0.004) in Model I. This association was attenuated after further adjustment for covariates in Model II but remained significant for memory function (−0.041, 95% CI −0.071, −0.012).

Table 2 Association between mean HbA1c level, visit-to-visit variability in HbA1c and memory decline using linear mixed models
Table 3 Association between mean HbA1c level, visit-to-visit variability in HbA1c and executive function decline using linear mixed models

We observed a potential dose–response relationship between the quartiles of HbA1c CV and the rate of cognitive decline. Participants in the highest quartile had a significantly accelerated rate of memory z score decline (−0.100 SD/year, 95% CI −0.197, −0.001) and executive function z score decline (−0.081 SD/year, 95% CI −0.126, −0.037) compared with that of the lowest quartile of HbA1c CV (p for trend <0.05). The results remained significant when further adjusted for education level, baseline BMI, smoking status, alcohol consumption status, disease status at baseline (including hypertension and diabetes) and HDL at baseline (ESM Fig. 1). When modelled as a continuous variable, a 1-SD increment in HbA1c CV was associated with both a higher rate of memory z score decline (−0.032 SD/year, 95% CI −0.049, −0.015) and executive function z score decline (−0.034 SD/year, 95% CI −0.084, −0.010) in the fully adjusted models. We did not observe the effect modification by sex in the current study (ESM Fig. 2).

HbA1c variability and cognitive decline among participants with diabetes

For diabetic participants (Fig. 2), each 1-SD increment in the CV of HbA1c was not associated with an increased rate of memory z score decline (−0.028 SD/year, 95% CI −0.065, 0.008) or executive function z score decline (0.018 SD/year, 95% CI −0.029, 0.064) (Model II). When modelled as a categorical variable (ESM Table 2), the highest quartile of HbA1c CV was associated with neither memory function (−0.023 SD/year, 95% CI −0.129, 0.082) nor executive function (0.064 SD/year, 95% CI −0.142, 0.270) in the fully adjusted model compared with the lowest quartile.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Subgroup analysis of the association between each SD increment in HbA1c variability and cognitive decline, stratified by baseline diabetes status

HbA1c variability and cognitive decline among participants without diabetes

For non-diabetic participants at baseline, each 1-SD increment in HbA1c CV was associated with a significantly higher rate of memory z score decline (−0.029 SD/year, 95% CI −0.052, −0.005) and executive function z score decline (−0.049 SD/year, 95% CI −0.079, −0.018) in the fully adjusted model. When the CV of HbA1c was modelled as a categorical variable, compared with the lowest quartile, the highest quartile of the CV of HbA1c was associated with a higher rate of memory decline (−0.055 SD/year, 95% CI −0.096, −0.013) and executive function decline (−0.088 SD/year, 95% CI −0.197, −0.022) in the fully adjusted model (ESM Table 2).

Sensitivity analysis

A similar pattern except for the significance of trend across quartiles of HbA1c CV was observed in a sensitivity analysis by excluding participants with missing values of cognitive assessment (ESM Table 3). ESM Fig. 3 shows that the distribution of the imputed data is generally similar to that of the original data. Sensitivity analyses by using the SD and VIM as indices of HbA1c variability did not substantially alter our findings (ESM Tables 4 and 5), but p values for the trend were not consistent with the main analysis. When we restricted the participants to those with four HbA1c measurements in the ELSA, a positive association between HbA1c variability and memory and executive function was still observed (ESM Table 6).

Discussion

In these two large population-based prospective cohorts (the HRS and ELSA) with a mean follow-up period of 10.48 years, we examined the association between HbA1c variability and cognitive decline. Overall, we observed that greater HbA1c variability was associated with steeper decline in cognitive function independent of mean HbA1c values among individuals without diabetes but not among individuals with diabetes. Sensitivity analyses did not materially change our results. Our findings could provide evidence of the detrimental effect of HbA1c variability and highlight the significance of steady glycaemic control.

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study of the association between long-term HbA1c variability and cognitive decline that analyses data from more than three cognitive function measurements over time. Moreover, prior epidemiological and clinical studies on this topic were mainly focused on individuals with type 2 diabetes mellitus [21, 22, 31, 32]. Two cross-sectional studies, both conducted among individuals with type 2 diabetes, reported significantly worse cognitive performance among participants with a greater degree of visit-to-visit glucose variability. Our study further extended the findings of a significant association between HbA1c VVV and cognitive decline among a non-diabetic population. Using data from two large prospective cohorts with biennial repeated cognitive measurements and a 10-year period of total follow-up, we were able to calculate the long-term glycaemic variability and trajectory of cognitive decline and investigate the association. Prior mechanics studies [33,34,35] also suggested a possible deleterious effect of glycaemic variability among individuals without diabetes, which was confirmed by our findings. Bancks et al found that higher intra-individual fasting glucose variability during young adulthood was associated with worse cognitive performances in midlife, and this association was stronger among individuals without diabetes [36]. These findings were consistent with the present result, although the cognitive tests used were different from those used in the current study. Positive associations were also observed between long-term glycaemic variability and mortality, CVD and type 2 diabetes [7, 37,38,39] among the non-diabetic population, which aligns with our findings.

The lack of a significant association of HbA1c variability with cognitive function decline among individuals with diabetes may be due to a number of factors. Bancks et al [36] assessed glucose variation before diabetes onset in the CARDIA study and observed that fasting glucose CV was more strongly associated with worse cognitive test scores among individuals without diabetes at the time of the cognitive test than among individuals with diabetes. This suggested that medication use for diabetes may disrupt the natural course of glycaemic variation and blunt the association of HbA1c variability with cognitive function. The relatively small number of participants with diabetes in the present study (n = 189 in the ELSA and n = 651 in the HRS) may restrict the power to detect a positive association. Moreover, previous post hoc analysis of clinical trials showed that significant associations between long-term glycaemic variation and outcomes (CVD or mortality) were only observed in the intensive glucose-lowering arm but not in the standard glucose-lowering arm [8, 40]. The latter is somehow consistent with our results, as elderly people with diabetes are unlikely to receive intensive glucose-lowering treatment according to the recommendations of the ADA [41]. Several studies have also reported that long-term HbA1c variability has a greater impact among individuals without diabetes, while short-term variability is a predictor among those with diabetes [33, 42]. Future studies are still needed to verify these observed associations.

Our findings also have implications for clinical practice. Individuals with large variability in HbA1c levels, although with absolute measurement level within the normal range, may have been neglected in regard to their risk of cognitive decline. Therefore, attention should be paid to the homeostasis of HbA1c levels in older adults. Measuring long-term HbA1c variability might help predict cognitive decline among individuals without diabetes. However, the causal relationship warrants verification in large clinical trials, and strategies to both maintain HbA1c absolute levels and reduce HbA1c variability merit further investigation.

The exact mechanism of the detrimental effect of glycaemic variability on cognitive decline remains unknown. Excessive insulin secretion caused by hyperglycaemia may result in peripheral or cerebral insulin resistance associated with neuronal vulnerability, neurodegeneration and further pathological lesions [43]. Impairment of insulin receptors and signalling in the brain may affect neuronal survival, astrocyte inflammatory cytokine secretion, nitric oxide-mediated vasodilation and cerebral perfusion [43]. A number of studies have indicated that glycaemic variability may cause apoptosis of pancreatic beta cells, which could result in the deterioration of glycaemic control and subsequent diabetic complications [44, 45].

Our study has several strengths, including the large population-based sample size, the inclusion of several metrics of HbA1c variability, the adoption of reliable tests of cognitive function, the appropriate statistical model and adjustment for mean HbA1c values. However, our findings should be interpreted in the context of the following limitations. First, because of the observational design, only associations, not causality, can be inferred from our results. Second, large intervals between HbA1c measurements (4 years) and relatively few measurements may restrict the generalisability of our findings. Third, although the HRS and ELSA were sister cohorts with a similar design, there are significant heterogeneities between the two sides of the Atlantic. Therefore, we used a meta-analysis approach with the random-effect model to combine the results from the two cohorts. Additionally, we excluded participants with less than three values of HbA1c in our study, but their characteristics were different from the included participants: the excluded participants were older, included a higher percentage of smokers, and had more depressive symptoms (ESM Table 1). Therefore, some caution is still necessary when extrapolating our findings to other populations. Fourth, we were unable to examine the relationship between long-term HbA1c VVV and the incidence of dementia/Alzheimer’s disease due to the lack of rigorous clinical diagnoses of dementia or Alzheimer’s disease during the follow-up of the HRS and ELSA. Finally, although we adjusted for several potential confounders, unmeasured variables such as genetic susceptibility, data for which were not available, may have affected our results.

Conclusion

In summary, our study provides evidence to support the association between long-term HbA1c variability and cognitive decline irrespective of the effect of mean HbA1c values among individuals without diabetes. Further studies are needed to determine whether decreasing glycaemic variability would benefit cognitive decline in the elderly.