Skip to main content

Children’s Names Contribute to Early Literacy a Linguistic and Social Perspective

  • Chapter
Perspectives on Language and Language Development

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 169.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Aram, D., & Levin, I. (2001). Mother-child joint writing in low SES: Sociocultural factors, maternal mediation, and emergent literacy. Cognitive Development, 16, 831–852.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aram, D., & Levin, I. (in press). The role of maternal mediation of writing to kindergartners in promoting literacy in school: A longitudinal perspective. Reading and Writing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arrow, A., Fletcher-Flinn, C. M., & Nicholson, T. (2003). “That’s my name”: Possible precursors to reading development. Poster presented at the Annual Conference of the Society for Scientific Study of Reading, Boulder, Colorado, USA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beers, J. W., & Henderson, E. H. (1977). A study of developing orthographic concept among first graders. Research in the Teaching of English, 2, 133–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloodgood, J. W. (1999). What’s in a name? Children’s name writing and literacy acquisition. Reading Research Quarterly, 34, 342–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burns, M. S., & Casbergue, R. (1992). Parent child interaction in a letter writing context. Journal of Reading Behavior, 24, 289–312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bus, A. G., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Pellegrini, A. D. (1995). Joint book reading makes for success in learning to read: A meta-analysis on intergenerational transmission of literacy. Review of Educational Research, 65, 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cassidy, K. W., Kelly, M. H., & Sharoni, L. J. (1999). Inferring gender from name phonology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 362–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clay, M. M. (1975). What did I write? Beginning writing behavior. City: Heinemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coulmas, F. (1989). The writing systems of the world. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deutsch, E. (1998). The effects of early reading instruction on emergent literacy: A comparison between the ultraorthodox “heder” and state preschool. Unpublished masters thesis, Sackler Faculty of Medicine, Dept. of Communication Disorders, Tel Aviv University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, M. A., Shaw, D., & Bell, M. (2000). Home literacy experiences and their influence on early literacy skills. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 65–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, M. A., Shaw, D., Moretti, S., & Page, J. (2003). Letter names, letter sounds and phonological awareness: An examination of kindergarten children across letters and of letters across children. Manuscript under review.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (1983). A critique of five studies related to letter-name knowledge and learning to read. In L. M. Gentile, M. L. Kamil, & J. S. Blanchard (Eds.), Reading research revisited (pp. 143–153). Columbus, OH: Merrill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (2002). Phases of acquisition in learning to read words and implications for teaching. British Journal of Educational Psychology: Monograph Series, 1, 7–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, N. (1975). Pictographs and prereading skills. Child Development, 46, 786–789.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferreiro, E. (1986). The interplay between information and assimilation in beginning literacy. In W. H. Teale, & E. Sulzby (Eds.), Emergent literacy (pp. 15–49). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferreiro, E., & Teberosky, A. (1982). Literacy before schooling. New York: Heinemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gombert, J. E., & Fayol, M. (1992). Writing in preliterate children. Learning and Instruction, 2, 23–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P., Juel, C., & Griffith, P. (1992). Reading, spelling and the orthographic cipher. In P. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 35–48). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M., & Hatano, G. (1999). Learning to read and write: A cross linguistic perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hiebert, E. H., & Adams, C. S. (1987). Fathers’ and mothers’ perceptions of their preschool children’s emergent literacy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 44, 25–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jamui, O. (2003). Name reading and writing among pre-and kindergartners. Unpublished masters thesis, School of Education, Tel Aviv University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., & Lieblich, I. (1975). Examination of the letter serial position effect in the “TOT” and the “don’t know” states. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 6, 539–541.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I., & Bus, A. (2003). How is emergent writing based on drawing? Analyses of children’s products and their sorting by children and mothers. Developmental Psychology, 39, ???.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I., Patel, S., Margalit, T., & Barad, N. (2002). Letter-names: Effect on letter saying, spelling and word recognition in Hebrew. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 269–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I., Ravid, D., & S. Rapaport (1996). Developing morphological awareness and learning to write: A two-way street. In T. Nunes (Ed.), Learning to read: An integrated view from research and practice, 1999 (pp. 77–104). Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I., Share, D. L., & Shatil, E. (1996). A qualitative-quantitative study of preschool writing: Its development and contribution to school literacy. In C. M. Levy & S. Ransdell (Eds.), The science of writing (pp. 271–293). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marchbanks, G., & Levin, H. (1965). Cues by which children recognize words. Journal of Educational Psychology, 56, 57–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martens, P. A. (1999). Mommy, how do you write “Sarah?”: The role of name writing in one child’s literacy. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 14, 5–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, J. M. (1980). When do children begin to read: An exploration of four year old children’s letter and word reading competencies. Reading Research Quarterly, 15, 203–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masonheimer, P. E., Drum, P. A., & Ehri, L. C. (1984). Does environmental print identification lead children into word reading? Journal of Reading Behavior, 16, 257–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C. (1999). The ABCs of the ABCs: The development of letter-name and letter-sound knowledge. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45, 285–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris, D. (1980). Beginning readers’ conception of word. In E. Henderson & J. Beers (Eds.), Developmental and cognitive aspects of learning to spell: A reflection of word knowledge (pp. 97–111). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris, D. (1983). Concept of word and phoneme awareness in the beginning of reading. Research in the Teaching of English, 17, 359–373.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peled-Haim, L. (2003). Name reading and literacy skills among pre-and kindergartners. Unpublished masters thesis, School of Education, Tel Aviv University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D. (2001). Learning to spell in Hebrew: Phonological and morphological factors. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 459–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D. (in press). Hebrew orthography and literacy. In R. M. Joshi & P. G. Aaaron (Eds.), Handbook of orthography and literacy. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Read, C. (1971). Pre-school children’s knowledge of English phonology. Harvard Educational Review, 41, 1–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Share, L. C. (in press). Knowing letter names and learning their sounds: A causal connection. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Share, D.L., & Gur, T. (1999). How reading begins: A study of preschoolers’ print identification strategies. Cognition and Instruction, 17, 177–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, L. C., & Levin, I. (1999). Learning to read and write in Hebrew. In M. Harris, & G. Hatano (Eds.), Learning to read and write: A cross linguistic perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolchinsky, L., & Teberosky, A. (1997). Explicitword segmentation and writing in Hebrewand Spanish. In C. Pontecorvo (Ed.), Writing development: An interdisciplinary view (pp. 77–98). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolchinsky, L., & Teberosky, A. (1998). The development of word segmentation and writing in two scripts. Cognitive Development, 13, 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., & Broderick, V. (1998). What’s in a name? Children’s knowledge about the letters in their own names. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70, 97–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R. & Kessler, B. (2003). The role of letter names in the acquisition of literacy. In H. W. Reese & R. Kail (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior, Vol. 31. San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (2004). The case of case: Children’s knowledge and use of upper-and lower-case letters. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 413–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., Kessler, B., & Bourassa, D. (2001). Children’s own names influence their spelling. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 555–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., Tincoff, R., & Richmond-Welty, E. D. (1996). Letter names help children to connect print and speech. Developmental Psychology, 32, 505–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., Tincoff, R., Rodriguez, K., Mouzaki, A., & Francis, D. J. (1998). The foundations of literacy: Learning the sounds of letters. Child Development, 69, 1524–1540.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., Weatherston, S., & Berch, D. (1994). The role of letter names in children’s learning of phoneme-grapheme relations. Applied Psycholinguistics, 15, 97–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Villaume, S. K., & Wilson, L. C. (1989). Preschool children’s explorations of letters in their own names. Applied Psycholinguistics, 10, 283–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Worden, P. E., & Boettcher, W. (1990). Young children’s acquisition of alphabet knowledge. Journal of Reading Behavior, 22, 277–295.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Iris Levin or Dorit Aram .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2005 Kluwer Academic Publishers

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Levin, I., Aram, D. (2005). Children’s Names Contribute to Early Literacy a Linguistic and Social Perspective. In: Ravid, D.D., Shyldkrot, H.BZ. (eds) Perspectives on Language and Language Development. Springer, Boston, MA . https://doi.org/10.1007/1-4020-7911-7_17

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics