Skip to main content

Bottom-Up Processing

  • Living reference work entry
  • First Online:
Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior
  • 34 Accesses

Synonyms

Data-driven processing; Local bias; Lower-level representation; Perceptually driven processing

Definition

In bottom-up processing, an organism’s perception of a stimulus relates directly to its perceptual features and does not impose a more abstract structure to the conceptualization of the object (as in top-down processing). Bottom-up processing is thus independent of prior experience and knowledge. It is thought to represent lower levels of information processing and to focus on detailed stimulus information compared to the more integrated higher-level information that reflects top-down processing (Rauss and Pourtois 2013). For example, when viewing a rectangle, one would see four lines intersecting rather than representing the rectangle as a depiction of a doorway or a window. Although bottom-up processing is thought to be largely automatic (Theeuwes 2010), the same could be said of some forms of top-down processing; therefore, the automatic/consciously controlled...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Ashby, F. G., & Ell, S. W. (2001). The neurobiology of human category learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 204–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biederman, I. (1987). Recognition-by-components: A theory of human image understanding. Psychological Review, 94, 115–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M., & Ferrari, P. F. (2010). Towards a bottom-up perspective on animal and human cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(5), 201–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deruelle, C., & Fagot, J. (1998). Visual search for global/local stimulus features in humans and baboons. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5(3), 476–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eaton, T., Hutton, R., Leete, J., Lieb, J., Robeson, A., & Vonk, J. (2018). Bottoms-up: Rejecting top-down human-centered approaches in comparative psychology. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 31, 1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eggermont, J. J. (2015). Animal models of auditory temporal processing. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 95(2), 202–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fagot, J., & Parron, C. (2012). Visual cognition in baboons: Attention to global and local stimulus properties. In O. F. Lazareva, T. Shimizu, & E. A. Wasserman (Eds.), How animals see the world: Comparative behavior, biology, and evolution of vision (pp. 371–385). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, H. D., Brosnan, S. F., Hopper, L. M., Lambeth, S. P., Schapiro, S. J., & Gosling, S. D. (2013). Developing a comprehensive and comparative questionnaire for measuring personality in chimpanzees using a simultaneous top-down/bottom-up design. American Journal of Primatology, 75(10), 1042–1053.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • French, R. M., Mareschal, D., Mermillod, M., & Quinn, P. C. (2004). The role of bottom-up processing in perceptual categorization by 3- to 4-month-old infants: Simulations and data. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(3), 382–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstone, R. L., & Barsalou, L. W. (1998). Reuniting perception and conception. Cognition, 65, 231–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hopper, L. M., Freeman, H. D., & Ross, S. R. (2016). Reconsidering coprophagy as an indicator of negative welfare for captive chimpanzees. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 176, 112–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jitsumori, M., & Matsuzawa, T. (1991). Picture perception in monkeys and pigeons: Transfer of rightside-up versus upside-down discrimination of photographic objects across conceptual categories. Primates, 32(4), 473–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kinchla, R. A., & Wolfe, J. M. (1979). The order of visual processing: “top-down,” “bottom-up,” or “middle-out”. Perceptual Psychophysics, 25, 225–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Qadri, M. A. J., & Cook, R. G. (2015). The perception of glass patterns by starlings (sturnus vulgaris). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(3), 687–693.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quinn, P. C., & Eimas, P. D. (1997). A reexamination of the perceptual to-conceptual shift in mental representations. Review of General Psychology, 1, 271–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rakison, D., & Oakes, L. M. (Eds.). (2003). Early category and concept development: Making sense of the blooming, buzzing confusion. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rauss, K., & Pourtois, G. (2013). What is bottom-up and what is top-down in predictive coding? Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, W. A., & Mazmanian, D. S. (1988). Concept learning at different levels of abstraction by pigeons, monkeys, and people. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 14(3), 247–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spinozzi, G., De Lillo, C., & Salvi, V. (2006). Local advantage in the visual processing of hierarchical stimuli following manipulations of stimulus size and element numerosity in monkeys (cebus apella). Behavioural Brain Research, 166(1), 45–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Theeuwes, J. (2010). Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. Acta Psychologica, 135, 77–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vonk, J., & Eaton, T. (2018). Personality in nonhuman animals: Comparative perspectives and applications. In V. Zeigler-Hill & T. Shackelford (Eds.), The Sage handbook of personality and individual differences (pp. 23–51). Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vonk, J., & Galvan, M. (2014). What do natural categorization studies tell us about apes and bears? Animal Behavior & Cognition, 1, 309–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jennifer Vonk .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Section Editor information

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2021 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this entry

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this entry

Vonk, J. (2021). Bottom-Up Processing. In: Vonk, J., Shackelford, T. (eds) Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_1279-1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_1279-1

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-47829-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-47829-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Reference Behavioral Science and PsychologyReference Module Humanities and Social SciencesReference Module Business, Economics and Social Sciences

Publish with us

Policies and ethics