Skip to main content

Distance-to-Death Research in Geropsychology

  • Reference work entry
  • First Online:
  • 52 Accesses

Synonyms

Distance-to-death and time-to-death; Terminal changes and time-to-death-related changes; Terminal decline and terminal drop; Time-to-death-related trajectory and time-to-death-related growth curves

Definition

In the broadest sense, distance-to-death research in geropsychology includes all kinds of examinations of associations between facets of psychological functioning and time-to-death. In a narrower sense, however, the term refers to the study of terminal changes in psychological functioning, that is, intraindividual changes that occur time-to-death related at the end of the individual’s lifespan. Up to the present, geropsychological distance-to-death research for the most part consists of studies of terminal decline and terminal drop in cognitive functioning and subjective well-being.

Distance-to-Death Research in Geropsychology

Across the past decades, research in geropsychology increasingly considered distance-to-death as indicator of psychological changes that unfold at...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   1,699.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD   1,799.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

References

  • Bäckman, L., & MacDonald, S. W. S. (2006). Death and cognition: Synthesis and outlook. European Psychologist, 11, 224–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P. B., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1979). History and rationale of longitudinal research. In J. R. Nesselroade & P. B. Baltes (Eds.), Longitudinal research in the study of behavior and development (pp. 1–39). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Birren, J. E., & Cunningham, W. R. (1985). Research on the psychology of aging: Principles, concepts and theory. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (2nd ed., pp. 3–34). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bosworth, H. B., & Siegler, I. C. (2002). Terminal change in cognitive function: An updated review of longitudinal studies. Experimental Aging Research, 28, 299–315.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Busse, E. W. (1969). Theories of aging. In E. W. Busse & E. Pfeiffer (Eds.), Behavior and adaptation in late life (pp. 11–32). Boston: Little and Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cadar, D., Stephan, B. C. M., Jagger, C., Johansson, B., Hofer, S. M., Piccinin, A. M., & Muniz-Terrera, G. (2015). The role of cognitive reserve on terminal decline: A cross-cohort analysis from two European studies: OCTO-Twin, Sweden, and Newcastle 85+, UK. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. doi:10.1002/gps.4366.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Charles, S. T., & Carstensen, L. L. (2009). Social and emotional aging. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 383–409.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cudeck, R., & Harring, J. R. (2007). Analysis of nonlinear patterns of change with random coefficient models. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 615–637.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Curran, P. J., Obeidat, K., & Losardo, D. (2010). Twelve frequently asked questions about growth curve modeling. Journal of Cognition and Development, 11, 121–136.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Deary, I. J., Whiteman, M. C., Starr, J. M., Whalley, L. J., & Fox, H. C. (2004). The impact of childhood intelligence on later life: Following up the Scottish Mental Survey of 1932 and 1947. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 130–147.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Erikson, E. H. (1997). The life cycle completed – Extended version. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerstorf, D., & Ram, N. (2013). Inquiry into terminal decline: Five objectives for future study. Gerontologist, 53, 727–737.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gerstorf, D., Ram, N., Estabrook, R., Schupp, J., Wagner, G. G., & Lindenberger, U. (2008a). Life satisfaction shows terminal decline in old age: Longitudinal evidence from the German Socio-Economic Panel Study (SOEP). Developmental Psychology, 44, 1148–1159.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gerstorf, D., Ram, N., Röcke, C., Lindenberger, U., & Smith, J. (2008b). Decline in life satisfaction in old age: Longitudinal evidence for links to distance-to-death. Psychology and Aging, 23, 154–168.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gerstorf, D., Ram, N., Mayraz, G., Hidajat, M., Lindenberger, U., Wagner, G. G., & Schupp, J. (2010). Late-life decline in well-being across adulthood in Germany, the United Kingdom, and the United States: Something is seriously wrong at the end of life. Psychology and Aging, 25, 477–485.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gerstorf, D., Ram, N., Lindenberger, U., & Smith, J. (2013). Age and time-to-death trajectories of change in indicators of cognitive, sensory, physical, health, social, and self-related functions. Developmental Psychology, 49, 1805–1821.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J., Wrosch, C., & Schulz, R. (2010). A motivational theory of life-span development. Psychological Review, 117, 32–60.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kaiser, R., Winning, K., Uter, W., Volkert, D., Lesser, S., Stehle, P., Kaiser, M. J., Sieber, C. C., & Bauer, J. M. (2010). Functionality and mortality in obese nursing home residents: An example of ‘risk factor paradox’? Journal of the American Medical Directors Association, 11, 428–435.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kalantar-Zadeh, K., Kilpatrick, R. D., Kuwae, N., & Wu, D. Y. (2005). Reverse epidemiology: A spurious hypothesis or a hardcore reality? Blood Purification, 23, 57–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kastenbaum, R. (2000). The psychology of death. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kleemeier, R. W. (1962). Intellectual changes in the senium. Proceedings of the Social Statistics Section of the American Statistical Association, 1, 290–295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kunzmann, U., Little, T. D., & Smith, J. (2000). Is age-related stability of subjective well-being a paradox? Cross-sectional and longitudinal evidence from the Berlin Aging Study. Psychology and Aging, 15, 511–526.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McArdle, J. J. (2009). Latent variable modeling of differences and changes with longitudinal data. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 577–605.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muniz-Terrera, G., van den Hout, A., Piccinin, A. M., Matthews, F. E., & Hofer, S. M. (2013). Investigating terminal decline: Results from a UK population-based study of aging. Psychology and Aging, 28, 377–385.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Palgi, Y., Shrira, A., Ben-Ezra, M., Spalter, T., Shmotkin, D., & Kavé, G. (2010). Delineating terminal change in subjective well-being and subjective health. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 65B, 61–64.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Pinquart, M. (2001). Age differences in perceived positive affect, negative affect, and affect balance in middle and old age. Journal of Happiness Studies, 2, 375–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ram, N., Gerstorf, D., Fauth, E., Zarit, S., & Malmberg, B. (2010). Aging, disablement, and dying: Using time-as-process and time-as-resources metrics to chart late-life change. Research in Human Development, 7, 27–44.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Riegel, K. F., & Riegel, R. M. (1972). Development, drop, and death. Developmental Psychology, 6, 306–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schilling, O. K. (2005). Cohort- and age-related decline in elder's life satisfaction: Is there really a paradox? European Journal of Ageing, 2, 254–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schilling, O. K., Wahl, H.-W., & Wiegering, S. (2013). Affective development in advanced old age: Analyses of terminal change in positive and negative affect. Developmental Psychology, 49, 1011–1020.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, I. C. (1975). The terminal drop hypothesis: Fact or artifact? Experimental Aging Research, 1, 169–185.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sliwinski, M. J., Hofer, S. M., Hall, C., Buschke, H., & Lipton, R. B. (2003). Modeling memory decline in older adults: The importance of preclinical dementia, attrition, and chronological age. Psychology and Aging, 18, 658–671.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sliwinski, M. J., Stawski, R. S., Hall, R. B., Katz, M., Verghese, J., & Lipton, R. B. (2006). On the importance of distinguishing pre-terminal and terminal cognitive decline. European Psychologist, 11, 172–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Small, B. J., & Bäckman, L. (1999). Time to death and cognitive performance. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 168–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smits, C. H. M., Deeg, D. J. H., Kriegsman, D. M. W., & Schmand, B. (1999). Cognitive functioning and health as determinants of mortality in an older population. American Journal of Epidemiology, 150, 978–986.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, N., Schilling, O. K., Wahl, H.-W., Beekman, A. T. F., & Penninx, B. W. J. H. (2013). Time-to-death-related change in positive and negative affect among older adults approaching the end of life. Psychology and Aging, 28, 128–141.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • White, N., & Cunningham, W. R. (1988). Is terminal drop pervasive or specific? Journal of Gerontology, 43, P141–P144.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, R. S., Beckett, L. A., Bienias, J. L., Evans, D. A., & Bennett, D. A. (2003). Terminal decline in cognitive function. Neurology, 60, 1782–1787.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, R. S., Segawa, E., Hizel, L. P., Boyle, P. A., & Bennett, D. A. (2012). Terminal dedifferentiation of cognitive abilities. Neurology, 78, 1116–1122.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Windsor, T. D., Gerstorf, D., & Luszcz, M. A. (2015). Social resource correlates of levels and time-to-death-related changes in late-life affect. Psychology and Aging, 30, 136–148.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wohlwill, J. F. (1970). The age variable in psychological research. Psychological Review, 77, 49–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yamaguchi, K. (1991). Event history analysis. Newbury Park: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Oliver K. Schilling .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer Science+Business Media Singapore

About this entry

Cite this entry

Schilling, O.K. (2017). Distance-to-Death Research in Geropsychology. In: Pachana, N.A. (eds) Encyclopedia of Geropsychology. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-287-082-7_125

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics