Skip to main content

Justice: A Neuroanthropological Account

  • Reference work entry
  • First Online:
Handbook of Neuroethics

Abstract

The central idea of this paper is that the directly experienced sense of justice and injustice is universal to peoples everywhere because the human brain is “wired” to experience empathy and harmony, and experience them associated with pleasure and injustice with pain. The sense of justice and injustice is shared with other big-brained social animals, particularly among social primates who share homologous neurophysiological structures with humanity. How the sense of justice plays out in societies depends upon neurocognitive, environmental, social structural, and cultural factors that impinge upon the development of each person’s perceptions, values, and personality. What a people consider as real, how they conceive of causation, and the information they credence about events will certainly vary across cultures. The sense of justice, both for the individual and among individuals in a community, is a principle ingredient in social institutions among more acephalous societies, while it may vary considerably among more complex and hierarchical societies. It is likely that all societies recognize the lack of empathy and sense of justice/injustice among abnormal individuals, and that appropriate action must be taken to protect society from actions that unsettle the balance of justice. Finally, by applying A.M. Hocart’s thinking to the issue, the individual’s sense of justice and injustice may become alienated from social procedures and juridical institutions, especially in highly complex, demographically diverse, and bureaucratized states.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 999.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 999.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Amit, E., & Greene, J. D. (2012). You see, the ends don’t justify the means: Visual imagery and moral judgment. Psychological Science, 23(8), 861–868.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, S. W., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1999). Impairment of social and moral behavior related to early damage in human prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 2(11), 1032–1037.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aureli, F., & de Waal, F. B. M. (Eds.). (2000). Natural conflict resolution. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandes, S. (1996). Empathy, narrative, and victim impact statements. The University of Chicago Law Review, 63(2), 361–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2011). The science of evil: On empathy and the origins of cruelty. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bekoff, M. (2007). The emotional lives of animals: A leading scientist explores animal joy, sorrow, and empathy – And why they matter. Novato: New World Library.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bekoff, M., & Pierce, J. (2009). Wild justice: The moral lives of animals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Dor, O. (2011). Law and art: Justice, ethics and aesthetics. Oxford: Routledge-Cavendish.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. J. R. (2007). Empathic dysfunction in psychopathic individuals. In T. F. D. Farrow & P. W. R. Woodruff (Eds.), Empathy in mental illness (pp. 3–16). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. J. R., & Blair, K. S. (2011). Empathy, morality, and social convention: Evidence from the study of psychopathy and other psychiatric disorders. In J. Docety & W. Ickes (Eds.), The social neuroscience of empathy (pp. 139–152). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. J. R., Mitchell, D., & Blair, K. (2005). The psychopath: Emotion and the brain. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bluehouse, P., & Zion, J. W. (1993). Hozhooji Naut’aanii: The Navajo justice and harmony ceremony. Mediation Quarterly, 10(4), 327–337. Reprinted in Nielsen, M. O., & Zion, J. W. (Eds.). (2005). Navajo Nation peacemaking: Living traditional justice (pp. 156–164). Tucson: University of Arizona Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bohannan, P. (1989). Justice and judgment among the Tiv. Prospect Heights: Waveland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick, M., Jha, A. P., Bylsma, L. M., Fabian, S. A., Solomon, P. E., & Prkachin, K. M. (2005). Viewing facial expressions of pain engages cortical areas involved in the direct experience of pain. NeuroImage, 25, 315–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braten, S. (2007). On being moved: From mirror neurons to empathy. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R. W., & Whiten, A. (1990). Tactical deception in primates: The 1990 database. Primate Report, 27, 1–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Changeux, J.-P. (2002). The physiology of truth: Neuroscience and human knowledge. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (2010). Almost chimpanzee: Searching for what makes us human, in rainforests, labs, sanctuaries, and zoos. New York: Henry Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooke, D. J. (1995). Psychopathic disturbance in the Scottish prison population: The cross-cultural generalizability of the hare psychopathy checklist. Psychology, Crime & Law, 2(2), 101–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooke, D. J. (1998). Psychopathy across cultures. In Psychopathy: Theory, research and implications for society (pp. 13–45). Springer, Netherlands.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csordas, T. J. (1990). Embodiment as a paradigm for anthropology. Ethos, 18, 5–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csordas, T. J. (Ed.). (1994). Embodiment and experience: The existential ground of culture and self. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • D’Andrade, R. (1995). Moral models in anthropology. Current Anthropology, 36(3), 399–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, R. C., & Smith, B. E. (1994). The effects of victim impact statements on sentencing decisions: A test in an urban setting. Justice Quarterly, 11(3), 453–469.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Oliveira, N. (2008). Husserl, Heidegger, and the task of a phenomenology of justice. Veritas, 53(1), 123–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Waal, F. B. M. (2006). Primates and philosophers: How morality evolved. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Waal, F. B. M. (2009). The age of empathy: Nature’s lessons for a kinder society. New York: Three Rivers.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Waal, F. B. M., & Aureli, F. (1996). Consolation, reconciliation, and a possible cognitive difference between macaque and chimpanzee. In A. E. Russon, K. A. Bard, & S. T. Parker (Eds.). Reaching into thought: The minds of the great apes (pp. 80–110). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Waal, F., & Lanting, F. (1997). Bonobo: The forgotten ape. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Decety, J., & Ickes, W. (Eds.). (2009). The social neuroscience of empathy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donovan, J. M., & Anderson, H. E. (2003). Anthropology and law. New York: Berghahn.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erez, E., & Tontodonato, P. (1992). Victim participation in sentencing and satisfaction with justice. Justice Quarterly, 9(3), 393–417.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erez, E., Kilchling, M., & Wemmers, J. (Eds.). (2011). Therapeutic jurisprudence and victim participation in justice: International perspectives. Durham: Carolina Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ethier, B. (2010). True crimes: New York City. Mechanicsburg: Stackpole.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farella, J. R. (1984). The main stalk: A synthesis of Navajo philosophy. Tucson: University of Arizona Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farrow, T., & Woodruff, P. W. R. (Eds.). (2007). Empathy in mental illness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flack, J. C., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2000). ‘Any animal whatever’: Darwinian building blocks of morality in monkeys and apes. In L. D. Katz (Ed.), Evolutionary origins of morality (pp. 1–29). Bowling Green: Imprint Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. (2003). The manifold nature of interpersonal relations: The quest for a common mechanism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 358, 517–528.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. (2005a). Embodied simulation: From neurons to phenomenal experience. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 4, 23–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. (2005b). “Being like me”: Self–other identity, mirror neurons and empathy. In S. Hurley & N. Chater (Eds.), Perspectives on imitation: From cognitive neuroscience to social science (Vol. 1, pp. 101–118). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. (2006). Intentional attunement: A neurophysiological perspective on social cognition and its disruption in autism. Brain Research, 1079, 15–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 396–403.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Eagle, M. N., & Migone, P. (2007). Intentional attunement: Mirror neurons and the neural underpinnings of interpersonal relations. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 55, 131–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garrels, S. R. (Ed.). (2011). Mimesis and science: Empirical research on imitation and the mimetic theory of culture and religion. East Lansing: Michigan State University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goulet, J.-G. (1994). Dreams and visions in other lifeworlds. In D. E. Young & J.-G. Goulet (Eds.), Being changed by cross-cultural encounters (pp. 16–38). Peterborough: Broadview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science, 293(5537), 2105–2108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. (2012). The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. New York: Pantheon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D. (2000). Wild minds: What animals really think. New York: Henry Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hocart, A. M. (1933). The progress of man: A short survey of his evolution, his customs and his works. London: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hocart, A. M. (1934). The role of consciousness in evolution. Psyche Annual, 14, 160–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hocart, A. M. (1970[1936]). Kings and councillors: An essay in the comparative anatomy of human society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollan, D. W., & Throop, C. J. (Eds.). (2011). The anthropology of empathy: Experiencing the lives of others in pacific societies. New York: Berghahn.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni, M., et al. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science, 286(5449), 2526–2528.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, P. L., Meltzoff, A. N., & Decety, J. (2005). How do we perceive the pain of others: A window into the neural processes involved in empathy. NeuroImage, 24, 771–779.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz, L. D. (2000a). Toward good and evil: Evolutionary approaches to aspects of human morality. In L. D. Katz (Ed.), Evolutionary origins of morality (pp. ix–xvi). Bowling Green: Imprint Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz, L. D. (Ed.). (2000b). Evolutionary origins of morality. Bowling Green: Imprint Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keysers, C. (2011). The empathic brain: How the discovery of mirror neurons changes our understanding of human nature. Amsterdam: Christian Keysers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Killen, M., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2000). The evolution and development of morality. In M. Killen & F. B. M. de Waal (Eds.), Natural conflict resolution (pp. 352–374). Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laughlin, C. D. (2011). Communing with the gods: Dream cultures and the dreaming brain. Brisbane: Daily Grail.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laughlin, C. D., & Allgeier, E. R. (1979). An ethnography of the So of northeastern Uganda. New Haven: Human Relations Area Files Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laughlin, C. D., & Throop, C. J. (2001). Imagination and reality: On the relations between myth, consciousness, and the quantum Sea. Zygon, 36(4), 709–736.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laughlin, C. D., & Throop, C. J. (2006). Cultural neurophenomenology: Integrating experience, culture and reality through fisher information. Journal Culture & Psychology, 12(3), 305–337.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laughlin, C. D., & Throop, C. J. (2008). Continuity, causation and cyclicity: A cultural neurophenomenology of time-consciousness. Time & Mind, 1(2), 159–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laughlin, C. D., & Throop, C. J. (2009). Husserlian meditations and anthropological reflections: Toward a cultural neurophenomenology of experience and reality. Anthropology of Consciousness, 20(2), 130–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laughlin, C. D., McManus, J., & d’Aquili, E. G. (1990). Brain, symbol and experience: Toward a neurophenomenology of human consciousness. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lepage, J. F., & Théoret, H. (2007). The mirror neuron system: Grasping other’s actions from birth? Developmental Science, 10(5), 513–529.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lonsdorf, E. V., Ross, S. R., & Matsuzawa, T. (Eds.). (2010). The mind of the chimpanzee: Ecological and experimental perspectives. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1964). Moral-analoges Verhalten der Tiere-Erkenntnisse heutiger Verhaltensforschung. Universitas, 19, 43–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lumpkin, T. W. (2001). Perceptual diversity: Is polyphasic consciousness necessary for global survival? Anthropology of Consciousness, 12(1–2), 37–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manning, R., Levine, M., & Collins, A. (2007). The Kitty Genovese murder and the social psychology of helping: The parable of the 38 witnesses. American Psychologist, 62(6), 555–562.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNeley, J. K. (1981). Holy wind in Navajo philosophy. Tucson: University of Arizona Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McParland, J., Eccleston, C., Osborn, M., & Hezseltine, L. (2011). It’s not fair: An interpretative phenomenological analysis of discourses of justice and fairness in chronic pain. Health, 15(5), 459–474.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N. (1985). The roots of social and cognitive development: Models of Man’s original nature. In T. M. Field & N. A. Fox (Eds.), Social perception in infants. Norwood: Ablex Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N. (2002). Elements of a developmental theory of imitation. In W. Prinz & A. Meltzoff (Eds.), The imitative mind: Development, evolution and brain bases (pp. 19–41). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1983a). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Development, 54, 702–709.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1983b). The origins of imitation in infancy: Paradigm, phenomena, and theories. In L. P. Lipsitt (Ed.), Advances in infancy research (Vol. 2). Norwood: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1997). Explaining facial imitation: A theoretical model. Early Development and Parenting, 6, 179–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, B. G. (2001). The problem of justice: Tradition and law in the Coast Salish world. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moghaddam, F. M. (2010). The new global insecurity. Santa Barbara: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moll, J., Zahn, R., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Krueger, F., & Grafman, J. (2005). The neural basis of human moral cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 799–809.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, S. F. (Ed.). (2005). Law and anthropology: A reader. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, J. M. (1976). Psychiatric labeling in cross-cultural perspective: Similar kinds of disturbed behavior appear to be labeled abnormal in diverse cultures. Science, 191(4231), 1019–1028.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, M. O., & Zion, J. W. (Eds.). (2005). Navajo Nation peacemaking: Living traditional justice. Tucson: University of Arizona Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niezen, R. (2010). Public justice and the anthropology of law. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Manique, J. (2003). The origins of justice: The evolution of morality, human rights, and the law. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, D. (2011). The moral lives of animals. New York: Bloomsbury.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pitchford, I. (2001). The origins of violence: Is psychopathy an adaptation? Human Nature Review, 1, 28–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Povinelli, E. A. (1993). Labor’s lot: The power, history and culture of aboriginal action. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rawls, J. (1971). A theory of justice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Resnik, J., & Curtis, D. E. (2011). Representing justice: The creation and fragility of courts in democracies. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rest, J., Narvaez, D., Bebeau, M. J., & Thoma, S. J. (1999). Postconventional moral thinking: A Neo-Kohlbergian approach. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., et al. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 131–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, M. J., Serra, E., Fadiga, L., & Gallese, V. (2008). The evolution of social cognition: Goal familiarity shapes monkeys’ action understanding. Current Biology, 18, 227–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosen, L. (1989). The anthropology of justice: Law as culture in Islamic society. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russon, A. E. (1996). Imitation in everyday use: Matching and rehearsal in the spontaneous imitation of rehabilitant orangutans. In A. E. Russon, K. A. Bard, & S. T. Parker (Eds.), Reaching into thought: The minds of the great apes (pp. 152–176). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russon, A. E., Bard, K. A., & Parker, S. T. (Eds.). (1996). Reaching into thought: The minds of the great apes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saver, J. L., & Damasio, A. R. (1991). Preserved access and processing of social knowledge in a patient with acquired sociopathy due to ventromedial frontal damage. Neuropsychologia, 29(12), 1241–1249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seitz, R. J., Nickel, J., & Azari, N. P. (2006). Functional modularity of the medial prefrontal cortex: Involvement in human empathy. Neuropsychology, 20(6), 743–751.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Tomer, R., Berger, B. D., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2003). Characterization of empathy deficits following prefrontal brain damage: The role of the right ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(3), 324–337.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shariat, S. V., Assadi, S. M., Noroozian, M., Pakravannejad, M., Yahyazadeh, O., Aghayan, S., Michie, C., & Cooke, D. (2010). Psychopathy in Iran: A cross-cultural study. Journal of Personality Disorders, 24(5), 676–691.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science, 303(5661), 1157–1162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Stephan, K. L., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature, 439, 466–469.

    Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, R. C. (1995). A passion for justice: Emotions and the origins of the social contract. Lanham: Rowman and Littlefield.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soroush, A. (2007). The beauty of justice. Centre for the Study of Democracy Bulletin, 14(1 & 2), 8–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, D., & Tifft, L. (2005). Restorative justice: Healing the foundations of our everyday lives (2nd ed.). Monsey: Willow Tree Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Throop, C. J. (2008a). On the problem of empathy: The case of Yap (Waqab), Federated States of Micronesia. Ethos, 36(4), 402–426.

    Google Scholar 

  • Throop, C. J. (2008b). From pain to virtue: Dysphoric sensations and moral sensibilities in Yap (Waqab), Federated States of Micronesia. Transcultural Psychiatry, 45, 253–286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Throop, C. J. (2009). ‘Becoming beautiful in the dance’: On the formation of ethical modalities in Yap, Federated States of Micronesia. Oceania, 79, 179–201.

    Google Scholar 

  • Throop, C. J. (2010a). Suffering and sentiment: Exploring the vicissitudes of experience and pain in Yap. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Throop, C. J. (2010b). Latitudes of loss: On the vicissitudes of empathy. American Ethnologist, 37(4), 281–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J.-P., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2003). Both of us disgusted in my insula: The common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron, 40, 655–664.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, B. A. O. (1980). Conclusion. In G. S. Stent (Ed.), Morality as a biological phenomenon: The presuppositions of sociobiological research (pp. 275–285). Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, J. Q. (1993). The moral sense. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witherspoon, G. (1977). Language and art in the Navajo universe. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham, R. W., de McGrew, W. C., Waal, F. B. M., & Heltne, P. G. (Eds.). (1994). Chimpanzee cultures. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yazzie, R. (1994). ‘Life comes from it’: Navajo justice concepts. New Mexico Law Review, 24, 175–190. Reprinted in Nielsen, M. O., & Zion, J. W. (Eds.). (2005). Navajo Nation peacemaking: Living traditional justice (pp. 42–58). Tucson: University of Arizona Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yazzie, R. (1995). Healing as justice: The American experience. Justice as Healing: A Newsletter on Aboriginal Concepts of Justice (pp. 1–5). Spring issue.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Charles D. Laughlin .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this entry

Cite this entry

Laughlin, C.D. (2015). Justice: A Neuroanthropological Account. In: Clausen, J., Levy, N. (eds) Handbook of Neuroethics. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-4707-4_135

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics