Skip to main content

Neurotheological Eudaimonia

  • Reference work entry
  • First Online:
Handbook of Neuroethics

Abstract

The Aristotelian concept of eudaimonia, variously translated as “happiness” or “flourishing,” has been a foundational principle of the Western philosophical tradition of virtue ethics. The theology of Thomas Aquinas transfigured the meaning of eudaimonia, introducing the concept of “infused” virtues. Rather than being acquired through habituation, these virtues can be understood metaphorically as removing a person’s “spiritual autism,” enabling second-person relatedness to God. This chapter applies these general principles to examine the everyday role of second-person relatedness in ethical formation, reviewing research into the neural conditions and concomitants of joint attention, and implications for a new understanding of virtue ethics.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 999.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 999.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Adolphs, R., & Janowski, V. (2011). Emotion recognition. In J. Decety & J. Cacioppo (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of social neuroscience (pp. 252–264). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Assal, G. (2001). Prosopagnosia. Bulletin de l’Académie Nationale de Médecine, 185(3), 525–535; discussion 535–536.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, M., Nettle, D., & Roberts, G. (2006). Cues of being watched enhance cooperation in a real-world setting. Biology Letters, 2(3), 412–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Belin, P. (2011). “Hearing voices”: Neurocognition of the human voice. In J. Decety & J. Cacioppo (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of social neuroscience (pp. 378–393). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, M. R., & Hacker, P. M. S. (2003). Philosophical foundations of neuroscience. Malden/Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meanings of words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, P. (2013). Just babies: The origins of good and evil. New York: Crown Publishing Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bodamer, J. (1947). Die prosop-agnosie. Archiv Für Psychiatrie Und Nervenkrankheiten, 179, 6–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, J. (2005). Joint attention and common knowledge. In N. Eilan et al. (Eds.), Joint attention: Communication and other minds (pp. 287–297). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 893–910.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ciaramelli, E., Muccioli, M., Làdavas, E., & Pellegrino, G. (2007). Selective deficit in personal moral judgment following damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2(2), 84–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costall, A., & Leudar, I. (2004). Where is the ‘theory’ in theory of mind? Theory & Psychology, 14(5), 623–646.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dalrymple, K. A., Corrow, S., Yonas, A., & Duchaine, B. (2012). Developmental prosopagnosia in childhood. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 29(5–6), 393–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donald, M. (2001). A mind so rare: The evolution of human consciousness. New York/London: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eilan, N. (2005). Joint attention, communication, and mind. In N. Eilan et al. (Eds.), Joint attention: Communication and other minds (pp. 1–33). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Falk, D. (2009). Finding our tongues: Mothers, infants and the origins of language. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farah, M. J., Levinson, K. L., & Klein, K. L. (1995). Face perception and within-category discrimination in prosopagnosia. Neuropsychologia, 33(6), 661–674.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari, M. D., & Sternberg, R. J. (1998). Self-awareness: Its nature and development. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari, P. F., Gallese, V., Rizzolatti, G., & Fogassi, L. (2003). Mirror neurons responding to the observation of ingestive and communicative mouth actions in the monkey ventral premotor cortex. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 17(8), 1703–1714.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, R. H., Gilovich, T., & Regan, D. T. (1993). Does studying economics inhibit cooperation? Journal of Economic Perspectives, 7(2), 159–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freiwald, W. A., Tsao, D. Y., & Livingstone, M. S. (2009). A face feature space in the macaque temporal lobe. Nature Neuroscience, 12(9), 1187–1196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119(2), 593–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gärdenfors, P. (2003). How homo became sapiens: On the evolution of thinking. Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heal, J. (2005). Joint attention and understanding the mind. In N. Eilan et al. (Eds.), Joint attention: Communication and other minds (pp. 34–44). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heilman, K. M., Leon, S. A., & Rosenbek, J. C. (2004). Affective aprosodia from a medial frontal stroke. Brain and Language, 89(3), 411–416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hein, G., & Singer, T. (2008). I feel how you feel but not always: The empathic brain and its modulation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18(2), 153–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herman, C. P., & Mack, D. (1975). Restrained and unrestrained eating. Journal of Personality, 43(4), 647–660.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hickok, G. (2009). Eight problems for the mirror neuron theory of action understanding in monkeys and humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(7), 1229–1243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, P. (2005). What puts jointness into joint attention? In N. Eilan et al. (Eds.), Joint attention: Communication and other minds (pp. 185–204). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hood, B. M., Willen, J. D., & Driver, J. (1998). Adult’s eyes trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants. Psychological Science, 9(2), 131–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hynes, C. A., Baird, A. A., & Grafton, S. T. (2006). Differential role of the orbital frontal lobe in emotional versus cognitive perspective-taking. Neuropsychologia, 44(3), 374–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni, M. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science, 286(5449), 2526–2528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni, M., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Gallese, V., Buccino, G., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Grasping the intentions of others with one’s own mirror neuron system. PLoS Biology, 3(3), e79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keenan, J. P., Gallup, G. G., & Falk, D. (2003). The face in the mirror: The search for the origins of consciousness. New York: Ecco.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirwan, M. L., White, L. K., & Fox, N. A. (2011). The emotion-attention interface: Neural, developmental, and clinical considerations. In J. Decety & J. Cacioppo (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of social neuroscience (pp. 227–242). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiyonari, T., Tanida, S., & Yamagishi, T. (2000). Social exchange and reciprocity: Confusion or a heuristic? Evolution and Human Behavior: Official Journal of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society, 21(6), 411–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Sparrow, S. S., Bildt, A., Cicchetti, D. V., Cohen, D. J., & Volkmar, F. R. (1999). A normed study of face recognition in autism and related disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(6), 499–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koenigs, M., Young, L., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Cushman, F., Hauser, M., & Damasio, A. (2007). Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements. Nature, 446(7138), 908–911.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kraut, R. (2012). Aristotle’s ethics. In The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2012/entries/aristotle-ethics/. Accessed 19 August 2013

  • Kuhl, P. K. (2011). Social mechanisms in early language acquisition: Understanding integrated brain systems supporting language. In J. Decety & J. Cacioppo (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of social neuroscience (pp. 649–667). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Legge, B. (2002). Can’t eat, won’t eat dietary difficulties and autistic spectrum disorders. London/Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Libet, B. (1985). Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8, 529–566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacDorman, K. F., & Ishiguro, H. (2006). The uncanny advantage of using androids in cognitive and social science research. Interaction Studies, 7(3), 297–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacIntyre, A. (2007). After virtue: A study in moral theory (3rd ed.). Notre Dame: University of Notre Dame Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGilchrist, I. (2009). The master and his emissary: The divided brain and the making of the western world. New Haven/London: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McInerny, R. M. (1993). The question of Christian ethics. Washington, DC: Catholic University of America Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meister, I. G., Wilson, S. M., Deblieck, C., Wu, A. D., & Iacoboni, M. (2007). The essential role of premotor cortex in speech perception. Current Biology: CB, 17(19), 1692–1696.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mori, M. (1970). The uncanny valley. Energy, 7(4), 33–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagel, T. (1974). What is it like to be a bat? The Philosophical Review, 83(4), 435–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niedenthal, P. M., Eelen, J., & Maringer, M. (2011). Embodiment and social cognition. In J. Decety & J. Cacioppo (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of social neuroscience (pp. 491–506). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nusbaum, H. C. (2011). Language and communication. In J. Decety & J. Cacioppo (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of social neuroscience (pp. 668–679). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events: A neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation Cérébrale, 91(1), 176–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perrett, D. I., Smith, P. A., Potter, D. D., Mistlin, A. J., Head, A. S., Milner, A. D., & Jeeves, M. A. (1985). Visual cells in the temporal cortex sensitive to face view and gaze direction. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Containing Papers of a Biological Character. Royal Society (Great Britain), 223(1232), 293–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinsent, A. (2012). The second-person perspective in Aquinas’s ethics: Virtues and gifts. New York/Abingdon: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Porter, J. (1992). The subversion of virtue: Acquired and infused virtues in the ‘Summa theologiae’. Annual of the Society of Christian Ethics, 12, 19–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, V., & Morris, P. (2004). Participants don’t need theories knowing minds in engagement. Theory & Psychology, 14(5), 647–665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roessler, J. (2005). Joint attention and the problem of other minds. In N. Eilan et al. (Eds.), Joint attention: Communication and other minds (pp. 230–259). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls, E. T. (2007). The representation of information about faces in the temporal and frontal lobes. Neuropsychologia, 45(1), 124–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2007). Dissociable prefrontal networks for cognitive and affective theory of mind: A lesion study. Neuropsychologia, 45(13), 3054–3067.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sperry, R. W. (1952). Neurology and the mind-brain problem. American Scientist, 40(2).

    Google Scholar 

  • Stump, E. (2010). Wandering in darkness: Narrative and the problem of suffering. Oxford: Clarendon.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Stump, E. (2011). The non-Aristotelian character of Aquinas’s ethics: Aquinas on the passions. Faith and Philosophy, 28(1), 29–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H. (Ed.). (1994). Constraints on language acquisition: Studies of atypical children. Hillsdale/Hove: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, P. (1980). Margaret Thatcher: A new illusion. Perception, 9(4), 483–484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1998). Reference: Intending that others jointly attend. Pragmatics & Cognition, 6(1–1), 229–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of interpersonal communication (pp. 321–372). Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Völlm, B. A., Taylor, A. N. W., Richardson, P., Corcoran, R., Stirling, J., McKie, S., Deakin, J. F. W., & Elliott, R. (2006). Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and empathy: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study in a nonverbal task. NeuroImage, 29(1), 90–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, D. D., Demos, K. E., & Heatherton, T. F. (2011). Staying in control: The neural basis of self-regulation and its failure. In J. Decety & J. Cacioppo (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of social neuroscience (pp. 360–377). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werle, M. A., Murphy, T. B., & Budd, K. S. (1993). Treating chronic food refusal in young children: Home-based parent training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26(4), 421–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimpory, D. C., Hobson, R. P., Williams, J. M. G., & Nash, S. (2000). Are infants with autism socially engaged? A study of recent retrospective parental reports. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 525–536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yin, R. K. (1969). Looking at upside-down faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81(1), 141–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Andrew Pinsent .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this entry

Cite this entry

Pinsent, A. (2015). Neurotheological Eudaimonia. In: Clausen, J., Levy, N. (eds) Handbook of Neuroethics. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-4707-4_100

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-4707-4_100

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-94-007-4706-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-007-4707-4

  • eBook Packages: Humanities, Social Sciences and Law

Publish with us

Policies and ethics