Cooperation, Coalition, and Alliances

  • Charlotte K. Hemelrijk
  • Ivan Puga-Gonzalez
  • Jutta Steinhauser
Living reference work entry

Abstract

In primates, cooperative acts have been observed such as communal rearing of offspring, cooperative mobbing of predators, supporting others in fights, and grooming others. Grooming builds up a social bond between the partners, helps in repairing relationships, and produces all kinds of benefits for the groomee, such as the reduction of parasites and of tension. Although the costs for the groomer are low, it has been regarded as an altruistic act and therefore is expected to be preferably directed toward kin or to be repaid by being reciprocated or exchanged for another service (e.g., support in fights, help in rearing offspring in the case of communal breeding systems, or access to some object, such as food, or some individual such as a female, an infant, or members of another group).

The formation of coalitions may result in the maintenance or in the increase of the dominance of an individual, in the expulsion of certain individuals from a group, in taking over a group, in the defense of the home range against other groups, in getting access to estrous females, and in the protection of an infant or adult female. The degree of cognition involved in coalitions is unclear.

Which members of a group cooperate differs from species to species; it may be influenced by genetic and social relationships, by the size and the composition of the group (the sex ratio), by the degree of competition, and by the distribution of food.

Keywords

Rhesus Macaque World Monkey Coalition Formation Japanese Macaque Alarm Call 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Altmann SA (1979) Baboon progressions: order or chaos? A study of one-dimensional group geometry. Anim Behav 27:46–80Google Scholar
  2. Aureli F, Cords M, van Schaik CP (2002) Conflict resolution following aggression in gregarious animals: a predictive framework. Anim Behav 64:325–343Google Scholar
  3. Barrett L, Henzi SP, Weingrill T, Lycett JE, Hill RA (1999) Market forces predict grooming reciprocity in female baboons. Proc R Soc Lond B 266:665–670Google Scholar
  4. Barrett L, Henzi SP, Weingrill T, Lycett JE, Hill RA (2000) Female baboons do not raise the stakes but they give as good as they get. Anim Behav 59:763–770PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Barrett L, Gaynor D, Henzi SP (2002) A dynamic interaction between aggression and grooming reciprocity among female chacma baboons. Anim Behav 63:1047–1053Google Scholar
  6. Barton R (1985) Grooming site preferences in primates and their functional implications. Int J Primatol 6:519–532Google Scholar
  7. Bercovitch FB (1988) Coalitions, cooperation and reproductive tactics among adult baboons. Anim Behav 36:1198–1209Google Scholar
  8. Bissonnette A, de Vries H, van Schaik CP (2009) Coalitions in male Barbary macaques, Macaca sylvanus: strength, success and rules of thumb. Anim Behav 78:329–335Google Scholar
  9. Bissonnette A, Bischofberger N, van Schaik CP (2011) Mating skew in Barbary macaque males: the role of female mating synchrony, female behavior, and male-male coalitions. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 65:167–182PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Boccia ML, Reite M, Laudenslager M (1989) On the physiology of grooming in a Pigtail Macaque. Physiol Behav 45:667–670PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Borries C, Sommer V, Srivastava A (1994) Weaving a tight social net: allogrooming in free-ranging female langurs (Presbytis entellus). Int J Primatol 15:421–443Google Scholar
  12. Caine NG (1984) Visual scanning by tamarins. Folia Primatol 43:59–67Google Scholar
  13. Call J (1999) The effect of inter-opponent distance on the occurrence of reconciliation in stumptail (Macaca arctoides) and rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Primates 40:515–523Google Scholar
  14. Call J, Aureli F, de Waal FBM (1999) Reconciliation patterns among stumptailed macaques: a multivariate approach. Anim Behav 58:165–172PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Chapais B (1981) The adaptiveness of social relationships among adult rhesus monkeys. PhD dissertation, University of Cambridge, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  16. Chapais B (1995) Alliances as a means of competition in primates: evolutionary, developmental and cognitive aspects. Yrbk Phys Anthropol 38:115–136Google Scholar
  17. Chapais B (1996) Competing through co-operation in nonhuman primates: developmental aspects of matrilineal dominance. Int J Behav Dev 19:7–23Google Scholar
  18. Chapais B (2004) How kinship generates dominance structures: a comparative perspective. In: Thierry B, Singh M, Kaumanns W (eds) How societies arise: the macaque model. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp 186–204Google Scholar
  19. Chapais B, Gauthier C (2004) Juveniles outrank higher-born females in groups of long-tailed macaques with minimal kinship. Int J Primatol 25:429–447Google Scholar
  20. Chapais B, Lecomte M (1995) Induction of matrilineal rank instability by the alpha male in a group of Japanese macaques. Am J Primatol 36:299–312Google Scholar
  21. Chapais B, Gauthier C, Prud’homme J, Vasey P (1997) Relatedness threshold for nepotism in Japanese macaques. Anim Behav 53:1089–1101Google Scholar
  22. Chapais B, Savard L, Gauthier C (2001) Kin selection and the distribution of altruism in relation to the degree of kinship in Japanese macaques (Macacfuscata). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 49:493–502Google Scholar
  23. Cheney DL (1987) Interactions and relationships between groups. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds) Primate societies. Chicago University Press, Chicago, pp 267–281Google Scholar
  24. Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (1981) Selective forces affecting the predator alarm calls of vervet monkeys. Behaviour 76:25–61Google Scholar
  25. Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (1985) Vervet alarm calls: manipulation through shared information. Behaviour 94:150–166Google Scholar
  26. Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (1990) How monkeys see the world. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  27. Cheney DL, Wrangham RW (1987) Predation. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds) Primate societies. Chicago University Press, Chicago, pp 227–239Google Scholar
  28. Combes SL, Altmann J (2001) Status change during adulthood: life-history byproduct or kin-selection based on reproductive value? Proc R Soc Lond B 268:1367–1373Google Scholar
  29. Connor RC (1995) Impala allogrooming and the parcelling model of reciprocity. Anim Behav 49:528–530Google Scholar
  30. Cooper MA (2004) Inter-group relationships. In: Thierry B, Singh M, Kaumanns W (eds) Macaque societies: a model for the study of social organization. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 204–208Google Scholar
  31. Cords M (1992) Post-conflict reunions and reconciliation in long-tailed macaques. Anim Behav 44:57–61Google Scholar
  32. Cords M (1995) Predator vigilance costs of allogrooming in wild blue monkeys. Behaviour 132:559–569Google Scholar
  33. Cords M (2000) Agonistic and affiliative relationships in a blue monkey group. In: Whitehead PF, Jolly CJ (eds) Old world monkeys. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 453–479Google Scholar
  34. Cords M (2002) Friendship among adult female blue monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis). Behaviour 139:291–314Google Scholar
  35. Cords M, Thurnheer S (1993) Reconciling with valuable partners by long-tailed macaques. Ethology 93:315–325Google Scholar
  36. Datta SB (1992) Effects of availability of allies on female dominance structure. In: Harcourt HA, de Waal FBM (eds) Coalitions and alliances in humans and in other animals. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 61–82Google Scholar
  37. Datta SB, Beauchamp G (1991) Effects of group demography on dominance relationships among female primates. I. Mother-daughter and sister-sister relations. Am Nat 138:201–226Google Scholar
  38. de Waal FBM (1978) Exploitative and familiarity dependent support strategies in a colony of semi-free living chimpanzees. Behaviour 66:268–312Google Scholar
  39. de Waal FBM (1982) Chimpanzee politics: sex and power among apes. Harper and Row, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  40. de Waal FBM (1984) Sex differences in the formation of coalitions among chimpanzees. Ethol Sociobiol 5:239–255Google Scholar
  41. de Waal FBM (1997) The chimpanzee’s service economy: food for grooming. Evol Hum Behav 18:375–386Google Scholar
  42. de Waal FBM (2000) Attitudinal reciprocity in food sharing among brown capuchin monkeys. Anim Behav 60:253–261PubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. de Waal FBM, Harcourt AH (1992) Coalitions and alliances: a history of ethological research. In: Harcourt AH, de Waal FBM (eds) Coalitions and alliances in humans and other animals. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 1–19Google Scholar
  44. de Waal FBM, Luttrell LM (1986) The similarity principle underlying social bonding among female rhesus monkeys. Folia Primatol 46:215–234PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. de Waal FBM, Luttrell LM (1988) Mechanisms of social reciprocity in three primate species: symmetrical relationship characteristics or cognition? Ethol Sociobiol 9:101–118Google Scholar
  46. de Waal FBM, van Hooff JARAM (1981) Side-directed communication and agonistic interactions in chimpanzees. Behaviour 77:164–198Google Scholar
  47. de Waal FBM, Yoshihara D (1983) Reconciliation and redirected affection in rhesus monkeys. Behaviour 85:224–241Google Scholar
  48. de Waal FBM, van Hooff JARAM, Netto WJ (1976) An ethological analysis of types of agonistic interaction in a captive group of Java-monkeys (Macaca fascicularis). Primates 17:257–290Google Scholar
  49. Di Bitetti MS (1997) Evidence for an important social role of allogrooming in a platyrrhine primate. Anim Behav 54:199–211Google Scholar
  50. Dugatkin LA (1997) Cooperation among animals: an evolutionary perspective. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  51. Dunbar RIM (1988) Primate social systems. Croom Helm, LondonGoogle Scholar
  52. Dunbar RIM (1991) Functional significance of social grooming in primates. Folia Primatol 57:121–333Google Scholar
  53. Dunbar RIM (1993) Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans. Behavioral and brain sciences 16:681–735Google Scholar
  54. Dunbar RIM (2003) The social brain: mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Annu Rev Anthropol 32:163–181Google Scholar
  55. Dunbar RIM, Sharman M (1984) Is Social Grooming Altruistic? Zeitschrift fuer Tierpsychologie 64:163–173Google Scholar
  56. Estrada A, Estrada, R, Ervin F (1977) Establishment of a Free-ranging Colony of Stumptail Macaques (Macaca arctoides): Social Relations I. Primates 18:647–676Google Scholar
  57. Fairbanks LA (1980) Relationships among adult females in captive vervet monkeys: testing a model of rank-related attractiveness. Anim Behav 28:853–859Google Scholar
  58. Fairbanks LA (1990) Reciprocal benefits of allomothering for female vervet monkeys. Anim Behav 40:553–562Google Scholar
  59. Frank RE, Silk JB (2009) Grooming exchange between mothers and non-mothers: the price of natal attraction in wild baboons (Papio anubis). Behaviour 146:889–906Google Scholar
  60. Franz C (1999) Allogrooming behaviour and grooming site preferences in captive bonobos (Pan paniscus): association with female dominance. Int J Primatol 20:525–546Google Scholar
  61. Fruteau C, Voelkl B, van Damme E, Noe R (2009) Supply and demand determine the market value of food providers in wild vervet monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:12007–12012PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Fruteau C, van de Waal E, van Damme E, Noe R (2011) Infant access and handling in sooty mangabeys and vervet monkeys. Anim Behav 81:153–161Google Scholar
  63. Goldberg TL, Wrangham RW (1997) Genetic correlates of social behaviour in wild chimpanzees: evidence from mitochondrial DNA. Anim Behav 54:559–570PubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe: patterns of behaviour. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA/LondonGoogle Scholar
  65. Goosen C (1975) After effects of allogrooming in pairs of adult stump-tailed macaques: a preliminary report. In: Kondo S, Kawai M, Ehara A (eds) Contemporary primatology (proceedings of fifth international congress primatology). Karger, Basel, pp 263–268Google Scholar
  66. Goosen C (1987) Social grooming in primates. In: Mitchell G, Erwin J (eds) Comparative primate biology. Part 2: behaviour, cognition, and motivation. Alan R. Liss, Inc., New York, pp 107–131Google Scholar
  67. Gouzoules S, Gouzoules H (1987) Kinship. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds) Primate societies. Chicago University Press, Chicago, pp 299–305Google Scholar
  68. Gros-Louis J, Perry S, Manson JH (2003) Violent coalitionary attacks and intraspecific killing in wild white-faced capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus). Primates 44:341–346PubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. Gumert MD (2007) Payment for sex in a macaque mating market. Anim Behav 74:1655–1667Google Scholar
  70. Hall KRL (1960) Social vigilance behaviour of the chacma baboon, Papio ursinus. Behaviour 16:261–294Google Scholar
  71. Hamilton WD (1964) The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I. J Theor Biol 7:1–16PubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Harcourt AH (1988) Alliances in contests and social intelligence. In: Byrne RW, Whiten A (eds) Machiavellian intelligence: social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans. Clarendon, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  73. Harcourt AH, de Waal FBM (1992) Coalitions and alliances in humans and other animals. Oxford University Press, Oxford, p 531Google Scholar
  74. Hare B, Tomasello M (2004) Chimpanzees are more skilful in competitive than in cooperative cognitive tasks. Anim Behav 68:571–581Google Scholar
  75. Hemelrijk CK (1990a) A matrix partial correlation test used in investigations of reciprocity and other social interaction patterns at a group level. J Theor Biol 143:405–420Google Scholar
  76. Hemelrijk CK (1990b) Models of, and tests for, reciprocity, unidirectional and other social interaction patterns at a group level. Anim Behav 39:1013–1029Google Scholar
  77. Hemelrijk CK (1991) Interchange of ‘altruistic’ acts as an epiphenomenon. J Theor Biol 153:137–139PubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Hemelrijk CK (1994) Support for being groomed in long-tailed macaques, Macaca fascicularis. Anim Behav 48:479–481Google Scholar
  79. Hemelrijk CK, Ek A (1991) Reciprocity and interchange of grooming and ‘support’ in captive chimpanzees. Anim Behav 41:923–935Google Scholar
  80. Hemelrijk CK, Luteijn M (1998) Philopatry, male presence and grooming reciprocation among female primates: a comparative perspective. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 42:207–215Google Scholar
  81. Hemelrijk CK, Puga-Gonzalez I (2012) An individual-oriented model on the emergence of support in fights, its reciprocation and exchange. PLoS ONE 7:e37271PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Hemelrijk CK, Klomberg TJM, Nooitgedagt JH, van Hooff JARAM (1991) Side-directed behaviour and recruitment of support in captive chimpanzees. Behaviour 118:89–102Google Scholar
  83. Hemelrijk CK, van Laere GJ, van Hooff JARAM (1992) Sexual exchange relationships in captive chimpanzees? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 30:269–275Google Scholar
  84. Hemelrijk CK, Meier CM, Martin RD (1999) ‘Friendship’ for fitness in chimpanzees? Anim Behav 58:1223–1229PubMedGoogle Scholar
  85. Hemelrijk CK, Wantia J, Daetwyler M (2003) Female co-dominance in a virtual world: ecological, cognitive, social and sexual causes. Behaviour 140:1247–1273Google Scholar
  86. Hemelrijk CK, Wantia J, Isler K (2008) Female dominance over males in primates: self-organisation and sexual dimorphism. PLoS ONE 3:e2678PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  87. Henzi SP, Barrett L (1999) The value of grooming to female primates. Primates 40:47–59Google Scholar
  88. Henzi SP, Barrett L (2002) Infants as a commodity in a baboon market. Anim Behav 63:915–921Google Scholar
  89. Higham JP, Maestripieri D (2010) Revolutionary coalitions in male rhesus macaques. Behaviour 147:1889–1908Google Scholar
  90. Hill DA (1994) Affiliative behaviour between adult males of the genus Macaca. Behaviour 130:293–308Google Scholar
  91. Hill DA (1999) Effects of provisioning on the social behaviour of Japanese and rhesus macaques: implications for socioecology. Primates 40:187–198PubMedGoogle Scholar
  92. Hill DH, Okayasu N (1995) Absence of youngest ascendancy in the dominance relations of sisters in wild Japanese macaques (Macacafuscata yakui). Behaviour 132:367–380Google Scholar
  93. Hrdy SB (1977) The langurs of Abu. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  94. Kapsalis E, Berman CM (1996) Models of affiliative relationships among free-ranging rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). 2. Testing predictions for three hypothesized organizing principles. Behaviour 133:1235–1263Google Scholar
  95. Keverne EB, Martensz ND, Tuite B (1989) Beta-endorphin concentrations in cerebrospinal fluid of monkeys are influenced by grooming relationships. Psychoendocrinology 14:155–161Google Scholar
  96. Koenig A (2000) Competitive regimes in forest-dwelling Hanuman langur females (Semnopithecus entellus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 48:93–109Google Scholar
  97. Lancaster JB (1971) Play-mothering: the relations between juvenile females and young infants among free-ranging vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). Folia Primatol 15:161–182Google Scholar
  98. Langergraber KE, Mitani JC, Vigilant L (2007) The limited impact of kinship on cooperation in wild chimpanzees. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:7786–7790PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  99. Lazaro-Perea C, Arruda MF, Snowdon CT (2004) Grooming as a reward? Social function of grooming between females in cooperatively breeding marmosets. Anim Behav 67:627–636PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  100. Maestripieri D (1993) Vigilance costs of allogrooming in Macaque mothers. Am Nat 141:744–753PubMedGoogle Scholar
  101. Maestripieri D (2000) Determinants of affiliative interactions between adult males and lactating females in pigtail macaques (Macaca nemestrina nemestrina). Ethology 106:425–439Google Scholar
  102. Majolo B, Ventura R, Koyama NF (2009) A statistical modelling approach to the occurrence and timing of reconciliation in wild Japanese macaques. Ethology 115:152–166Google Scholar
  103. Manson JH, Navarrete CD, Silk JB, Perry S (2004) Time-matched grooming in female primates? New analysis from two species. Anim Behav 67:493–500Google Scholar
  104. Massey A (1977) Agonistic aids and kinship in a group of pigtail macaques. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 12:31–40Google Scholar
  105. Matsumura S (1996) Postconflict affiliative contacts between former opponents among wild Moor macaques (Macaca maurus). Am J Primatol 38:211–219Google Scholar
  106. Matsumura S (1997) Mothers in a wild group of moor macaques (Macaca maurus) are more attractive to other group members when holding their infants. Folia Primatol 68:77–85PubMedGoogle Scholar
  107. McGrew WC (1992) Chimpanzee material culture. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  108. McKenna JJ (1979) The evolution of allomothering behavior among Colobine monkeys: function and opportunism in evolution. Am Anthropol 81:818–840Google Scholar
  109. Meier C, Hemelrijk CK, Martin RD (2000) Paternity determination, genetic characterization and social correlates in a captive group of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Primates 41:175–183Google Scholar
  110. Michael RP, Bonsall RW, Zumpe D (1978) Consort bonding and operant behavior by female rhesus monkeys. Comp Physiol Psychol 92:837–845Google Scholar
  111. Muroyama Y (1991) Mutual reciprocity of grooming in female Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Behaviour 119:161–170Google Scholar
  112. Muroyama Y (1994) Exchange of grooming for allomothering in female patas monkeys. Behaviour 128:103–119Google Scholar
  113. Nishida T (1983) Alpha status and agonistic alliances in wild chimpanzees. Primates 24:318–336Google Scholar
  114. Noë R (1990) A veto game played by baboons: a challenge to the use of the prisoner’s dilemma as a paradigm for reciprocity and cooperation. Anim Behav 39:78–90Google Scholar
  115. Noë R (1992) Alliance formation among male baboons: shopping for profitable partners. In: Harcourt AH, de Waal FBM (eds) Coalitions and alliances in humans and other animals. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 285–321Google Scholar
  116. Noë R, Sluijter AA (1995) Which adult male Savanna baboons form coalitions? Int J Primatol 16:77–105Google Scholar
  117. O'Brien TG (1993) Allogrooming behaviour among adult female wedge-capped capuchin monkeys. Anim Behav 46:499–510Google Scholar
  118. Packer C (1977) Reciprocal altruism. Nature 265:441–443Google Scholar
  119. Palombit RA, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (1997) The adaptive value of ‘friendships’ to female baboons: experimental and observational evidence. Anim Behav 54:599–614PubMedGoogle Scholar
  120. Palombit RA, Cheney DL, Fisher J, Johnson S, Rendall D, Seyfarth R, Silk J, van Schaik CP, Janson CH (2000) Male infanticide and defense of infants in chacma baboons. In: Infanticide by males and its implications. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 123–152Google Scholar
  121. Parish AR (1994) Sex and food control in the ‘uncommon chimpanzee’: How bonobo females overcome a phylogenetic legacy of male dominance. Ethol Sociobiol 15:157–179Google Scholar
  122. Parr LA, Matheson MD, Bernstein IS, De Waal FBM (1997) Grooming down the hierarchy: allogrooming in captive brown capuchin monkeys, Cebusapella. Anim Behav 54:361–367PubMedGoogle Scholar
  123. Paxton R, Basile BM, Adachi I, Suzuki WA, Wilson ME, Hampton RR (2010) Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) rapidly learn to select dominant individuals in videos of artificial social interactions between unfamiliar conspecifics. J Comp Psychol 124:395–401PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  124. Payne HFP, Lawes MJ, Henzi SP (2003) Competition and the exchange of grooming among female samango monkeys (Cercopithecus erythrarchus). Behaviour 140:453–471Google Scholar
  125. Petit O, Thierry B (1994) Aggressive and peaceful interventions in conflicts in Tonkean macaques. Anim Behav 48:1427–1436Google Scholar
  126. Prud’homme J, Chapais B (1993) Rank relations among sisters in semi-free ranging Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus). Int J Primatol 14:405–420Google Scholar
  127. Prud’homme J, Chapais B (1996) Development of intervention behaviour in Japanese macaques: testing the targeting hypothesis. Int J Primatol 17:429–443Google Scholar
  128. Puga-Gonzalez I, Hildenbrandt H, Hemelrijk CK (2009) Emergent patterns of social affiliation in primates, a model. Plos Comput Biol 5:e1000630. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000630PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  129. Range F, Noe R (2005) Can simple rules account for the pattern of triadic interactions in juvenile and adult sooty mangabeys? Anim Behav 69:445–452Google Scholar
  130. Rapaport LG (2001) Food transfer among adult lion tamarins: mutualism, reciprocity or one-sided relationships? Int J Primatol 22:611–629Google Scholar
  131. Rhine RJ, Westland BJ (1981) Adult male positioning in baboon progressions: order and chaos revisited. Folia Primatol 35:77–116PubMedGoogle Scholar
  132. Roberts G, Sherratt TN (1998) Development of cooperative relationships through increasing investment. Nature 394:175–179PubMedGoogle Scholar
  133. Robinson JG (1988) Group size in wedge-capped capuchin monkeys Cebusolivaceus and the reproductive success of males and females. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 23:187–197Google Scholar
  134. Ross C, MacLarnon A (2000) The evolution of non-maternal care in anthropoid primates: a test of the hypotheses. Folia Primatol 71:93–113PubMedGoogle Scholar
  135. Rowell TE (1968) Grooming by adult baboons in relation to reproductive cycles. Anim Behav 16:585–588PubMedGoogle Scholar
  136. Rowell TE, Wilson C, Cords M (1991) Reciprocity and partner preference in grooming of female blue monkeys. Int J Primatol 12:319–336Google Scholar
  137. Ruiter JR (1998) Relatedness of matrilines, dispersing males and social groups in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Proc R Soc Lond 265:79–87Google Scholar
  138. Schino G (2001) Grooming, competition and social rank among female primates: a meta-analysis. Anim Behav 62:265–271Google Scholar
  139. Schino G (2007) Grooming and agonistic support: a meta-analysis of primate reciprocal altruism. Behav Ecol 18:115–120Google Scholar
  140. Schino G, Aureli F (2008a) Grooming reciprocation among female primates: a meta-analysis. Biol Lett 4:9–11PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  141. Schino G, Aureli F (2008b) Trade-offs in primate grooming reciprocation: testing behavioural flexibility and correlated evolution. Biol J Linn Soc 95:439–446Google Scholar
  142. Schino G, Aureli F (2009) Reciprocal altruism in primates: partner choice, cognition, and emotions. Adv Stud Behav 39:45–69Google Scholar
  143. Schino G, Scucchi S, Maestripieri D, Turillazzi PG (1988) Allogrooming as a tension-reduction mechanism: a behavioral approach. Am J Primatol 16:43–50Google Scholar
  144. Schino G, Ventura R, Troisi A (2003) Grooming among female Japanese macaques: distinguishing between reciprocation and interchange. Behav Ecol 14:887–891Google Scholar
  145. Schino G, Tiddi B, Di Sorrentino EP (2006) Simultaneous classification by rank and kinship in Japanese macaques. Anim Behav 71:1069–1074Google Scholar
  146. Schino G, di Sorrentino EP, Tiddi B (2007) Grooming and coalitions in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata): partner choice and the time frame of reciprocation. J Comp Psychol 121:181–188PubMedGoogle Scholar
  147. Schino G, Di Giuseppe F, Visalberghi E (2009) Grooming, rank, and agonistic support in tufted capuchin monkeys. Am J Primatol 71:101–105. doi:10.1002/ajp.20627PubMedGoogle Scholar
  148. Schuelke O, Bhagavatula J, Vigilant L, Ostner J (2010) Social bonds enhance reproductive success in male macaques. Curr Biol 20:2207–2210Google Scholar
  149. Seyfarth RM (1976) Social relationships among adult female baboons. Anim Behav 24:917–938Google Scholar
  150. Seyfarth RM (1977) A model of social grooming among adult female monkeys. J Theor Biol 65:671–798PubMedGoogle Scholar
  151. Seyfarth RM (1978a) Social relationships among adult male and female baboons. I. Behaviour during sexual consortship. Behaviour 64:204–226Google Scholar
  152. Seyfarth RM (1978b) Social relationships among adult male and female baboons. II. Behaviour throughout the female reproductive cycle. Behaviour 64:227–247Google Scholar
  153. Seyfarth RM (1980) The distribution of grooming and related behaviours among adult female vervet monkeys. Anim Behav 28:798–813Google Scholar
  154. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (1984) Grooming, alliances and reciprocal altruism in vervet monkeys. Nature 308:541–543PubMedGoogle Scholar
  155. Shutt K, MacLarnon A, Heistermann M, Semple S (2007) Grooming in Barbary macaques: better to give than to receive? Biol Lett 3:231–233PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  156. Silk JB (1982) Altruism among female Macaca radiata: explanations and analysis of patterns of grooming and coalition formation. Behaviour 79:162–188Google Scholar
  157. Silk JB (1992a) The patterning of intervention among male bonnet macaques: reciprocity, revenge, and loyalty. Curr Anthropol 33:318–325Google Scholar
  158. Silk JB (1992b) Patterns of intervention in agonistic contests among male bonnet macaques. In: Harcourt AH, Waal FBM (eds) Coalitions and alliances in humans and other animals. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  159. Silk JB (1999) Male bonnet macaques use information about third-party rank relationships to recruit allies. Anim Behav 58:45–51PubMedGoogle Scholar
  160. Silk JB (2002) The form and function of reconciliation in primates. Ann Rev Anthropol 31:21–44Google Scholar
  161. Silk JB, Alberts SC, Altmann J (2003) Social bonds of female baboons enhance infant survival. Science 302:1231–1234PubMedGoogle Scholar
  162. Silk JB, Alberts SC, Altmann J (2004) Patterns of coalition formation by adult female baboons in Amboseli, Kenya. Anim Behav 67:573–582Google Scholar
  163. Silk JB, Beehner JC, Bergman TJ, Crockford C, Engh AL, Moscovice LR, Wittig RM, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2009) The benefits of social capital: close social bonds among female baboons enhance offspring survival. Proc R Soc B-Biol Sci 276:3099–3104Google Scholar
  164. Silk JB, Beehner JC, Bergman TJ, Crockford C, Engh AL, Moscovice LR, Wittig RM, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2010) Strong and consistent social bonds enhance the longevity of female baboons. Curr Biol 20:1359–1361. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.05.067PubMedGoogle Scholar
  165. Slater KY, Schaffner CM, Aureli F (2007) Embraces for infant handling in spider monkeys: evidence for a biological market? Anim Behav 74:455–461Google Scholar
  166. Smuts BB (1985a) Male-male competition for mates. In: Smuts BB (ed) Sex and friendship in baboons. Aldine Publishing Company, New York, pp 123–157Google Scholar
  167. Smuts BB (1985b) Sex and friendship in baboons. Aldine, HawthorneGoogle Scholar
  168. Smuts BB (1987) Sexual competition and mate choice. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds) Primate societies. Chicago University Press, Chicago, pp 385–399Google Scholar
  169. Stammbach E (1978) Social differentiation in groups of captive female hamadryas baboons. Behaviour 67:322–338Google Scholar
  170. Stammbach E (1988) Group responses to specially skilled individuals in a Macaca fascicularis group. Behaviour 107:241–266Google Scholar
  171. Stammbach E, Kummer H (1982) Individual contributions to a dyadic interaction: an analysis of baboon grooming. Anim Behav 30:964–971Google Scholar
  172. Struhsaker TT (1976) Auditory communication among vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). In: Altmann SA (ed) Social communication among primates. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 3–14Google Scholar
  173. Terry RL (1970) Primate grooming as a tension reduction mechanism. J Psychol 76:129–136PubMedGoogle Scholar
  174. Thierry B (1990) Feedback loop between kinship and dominance: the macaque model. J Theor Biol 145:511–521PubMedGoogle Scholar
  175. Thierry B (2000) Covariation of conflict management patterns across Macaque species. In: Aureli F, de Waal FBM (eds) Natural conflict resolution. University of California Press, Berkeley, pp 106–128Google Scholar
  176. Thierry B (2004) Social epigenesis. In: Thierry B, Singh M, Kaumanns W (eds) Macaque societies: a model for the study of social organisation. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 267–289Google Scholar
  177. Thierry B, Iwaniuk AN, Pellis SM (2000) The influence of phylogeny on the social behaviour of macaques (Primates: Cercopithecidae, genus Macaca). Ethology 106:713–728Google Scholar
  178. Tiddi B, Aureli F, Schino G (2012) Grooming up the hierarchy: the exchange of grooming and rank-related benefits in a new world primate. PLoS ONE 7(5): e36641. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0036641PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  179. Tomasello M, Call J, Gluckman A (1997) The comprehension of novel communicative signs by apes and human children. Child Dev 68:1067–1081PubMedGoogle Scholar
  180. Trivers RL (1971) The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quart Rev Biol 46:35–57Google Scholar
  181. Uehara S, Hiraiwa-Hasegawa M, Hosaka K, Hamai M (1994) The fate of defeated alpha male chimpanzees in relation to their social networks. Primates 35:49–55Google Scholar
  182. vanSchaik CP, Pandit SA, Vogel ER (2004) A model for within-group coalitionary aggression among males. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 57:101–109Google Scholar
  183. Ventura R, Majolo B, Koyama NF, Hardie S, Schino G (2006) Reciprocation and interchange in wild Japanese macaques: grooming, cofeeding, and agonistic support. Am J Primatol 68:1138–1149PubMedGoogle Scholar
  184. Vervaecke H, De Vries H, Van Elsacker L (2000) The pivotal role of rank in grooming and support behaviour in a captive group of bonobos (Pan paniscus). Behaviour 137:1463–1485Google Scholar
  185. Walters J (1980) Interventions and the development of dominance relationships in female baboons. Folia Primatol 34:61–89PubMedGoogle Scholar
  186. Watanabe K (1979) Alliance formation in a free-ranging troop of Japanese macaques. Primates 20:459–474Google Scholar
  187. Watts DP (1994) Social relationships of immigrant and resident female mountain gorillas. 2. Relatedness, residence, and relationships between females. Am J Primatol 32:13–30Google Scholar
  188. Watts DP (1995a) Post-conflict social events in wild mountain gorillas (Mammalia, Hominoidea). I. Social interactions between opponents. Ethology 100:139–157Google Scholar
  189. Watts DP (1995b) Post-conflict social events in wild mountain gorillas. 2. Redirection, side direction, and consolation. Ethology 100:158–174Google Scholar
  190. Watts DP (1997) Agonistic interventions in wild mountain gorilla groups. Behaviour 134:23–57Google Scholar
  191. Watts DP (2000a) Grooming between male chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park. I. Partner number and diversity and grooming reciprocity. Int J Primatol 21:189–210Google Scholar
  192. Watts DP (2000b) Grooming between male chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park. II. Influence of male rank and possible competition for partners. Int J Primatol 21:211–238Google Scholar
  193. Watts DP (2002) Reciprocity and interchange in the social relationships of wild male chimpanzees. Behaviour 139:343–370Google Scholar
  194. Watts DP (2004) Intracommunity coalitionary killing of an adult male chimpanzee at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. Int J Primatol 25:507–521Google Scholar
  195. Widdig A, Streich WJ, Tembrock G (2000) Coalition formation among male Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus). Am J Primatol 50:37–51PubMedGoogle Scholar
  196. Widdig A, Streich W, Nuernberg P, Croucher P, Bercovitch FB, Krawczak M (2006) Paternal kin bias in the agonistic interventions of adult female rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 61:205–214Google Scholar
  197. Wilkinson GS (1988) Reciprocal altruism in bats and other mammals. Ethol Sociobiol 9:85–100Google Scholar
  198. Wilson ML, Wrangham RW (2003) Intergroup relations in chimpanzees. Annu Rev Anthropol 32:363–392Google Scholar
  199. Wilson ML, Wallauer WR, Pusey AE (2004) New cases of intergroup violence among chimpanzees in Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Int J Primatol 25:523–549Google Scholar
  200. Wrangham RW (1980) An ecological model of female-bonded primate groups. Behaviour 75:262–300Google Scholar
  201. Wrangham RW (1999) Evolution of coalitionary killing. Yrbk Phys Anthropol 42:1–30Google Scholar
  202. Zamma K (2002) Grooming site preferences determined by lice infection among Japanese macaques in Arashiyama. Primates 43:41–49PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Charlotte K. Hemelrijk
    • 1
  • Ivan Puga-Gonzalez
    • 2
  • Jutta Steinhauser
    • 3
  1. 1.Behavioural Ecology and Self-organization, Centre for Ecological and Evolutionary StudiesUniversity of GroningenGroningenThe Netherlands
  2. 2.Behavioural Ecology and Self-organization, Centre for Ecological and Evolutionary StudiesUniversity of GroningenGroningenThe Netherlands
  3. 3.Behavioural Ecology and Self-organization, Centre for Ecological and Evolutionary StudiesUniversity of GroningenGroningenThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations