Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior

Living Edition
| Editors: Jennifer Vonk, Todd Shackelford

A-Not-B Problem

  • Britta OsthausEmail author
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_1096-1

Keywords

Detour Task Object Permanence Detour Behavior Osthaus Visual Displacement 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Synonyms

Definition

The A-not-B error arises from the difficulty of switching an action directed toward one location toward a new location. The original set-up, called visible displacement, involves hiding a toy in location A while an infant is watching. The child is then allowed to search for it. If they retrieve the toy it is seen as an indication of object permanence – the understanding that objects continue to exist even if they are no longer perceived. After a number of repeats of this hide and seek procedure, the toy is then obviously hidden in a different location, B. Up to a certain developmental stage, infants persevere in searching in location A instead of B. This is called the A-not-B error. This behavior can be observed in reaching tasks, but also in larger spatial set-ups that require detours.

Introduction

When Piaget published his theory of child development in 1937 (1954 in English), it quickly became absorbed by comparative psychologists because it offered a suitable framework for interspecies comparison. There are two reasons for this: first, Piaget was originally trained as a zoologist (the subject of his Ph.D. thesis was the snail), so his ideas are expressed within a broadly biological frame; and second, he denied that language develops prior to cognition, so his experimental tasks were designed to test cognition without relying on language. Much of his theorizing on early development was based on observable, nonverbal behavior, which makes it ideal for the study of animals (Pepperberg 2002).

According to Piaget, the A-not-B error occurs in Stage IV of the sensorimotor period (0–24 months) of human development. During stages I, II, and III infants acquire fundamental perceptual and motor abilities enabling them to perform search behavior. They are not yet able to search for an object that is no longer visible: it ceases to exist in the perceptual world of the infant and they therefore do not possess the concept of object permanence. Stage IV (from around 8 months) is defined by the ability of the infant to search for an object that has moved (or has been moved) out of sight. Here children are very likely to show the so-called A-not-B error, or Stage IV error, in which they continue to search at a previous hiding place (A) although the object was obviously hidden at a new place (B) (see Diamond 1990, for a review). A number of factors can affect the occurrence of the perseveration error, such as the delay between the hiding and retrieval, the number of possible hiding places, the motor requirements, and obviously the age of the infant (Markovitch and Zelazo 1999).

It was initially thought that children in Stage IV (approximately between 8 and 12 months) simply do not understand the concept of an object as continuing to exist out of sight, and in one place only. In certain set-ups, the error could be caused by associative learning as a repeated behavior is reinforced. At Stage V (from around 12 months), the A-not-B error no longer occurs.

Comparative Studies

Piaget himself was the first to apply his theory to animal behavior, albeit at a theoretical level (Piaget 1971). The first published experimental Piagetian animal research was done in 1971 by Gruber, Girgus, and Banuazizi who investigated the development of object permanence in cats. And in addition to a large number of studies with primates, object permanence has been studied in dogs and wolves (Gagnon and Doré 1994), chicks (Etienne 1973), hamsters (Thinus-Blanc and Scardigli 1981), and magpies (Pollok et al. 2000). Vauclair (1996) gives a detailed account of published studies in this area, as do Cacchione and Rakoczy (2017).

This order of the developmental stages has been applied to animals as it provides a valid and reliable base for a comparison among species. Especially in the area of object permanence, animals can be grouped by the highest stage they can master.

Etienne (1973) stated that animals react in three different ways to the disappearance of objects: animals like insects, spiders, and some lower vertebrates apply special devices or stereotyped movements to find an object of immediate survival value; most birds and some mammals, like rabbits, possess either learned or unconditional responses they apply to find an object that has disappeared, but with no persistence; some mammalian carnivores, primates, and birds of the family Corvidae show spontaneous and active search behavior in response to the disappearance of an object. This third group’s performance corresponds to at least Stage IV of Piaget’s ranking of object permanence performance.

When considering the current literature on A-not-B errors in nonhuman animals, it has to be taken into account that the task set-up often varies from Piaget’s original one. Most studies use variations of Uzgiris and Hunt’s (1989) sequence of tests where the task for successive visible displacement involves the hiding of the target (treat or toy) out of sight in one location before it is moved by the experimenter in plain sight to a second location. In the original tests by Piaget, the target was hidden and retrieved from location A several times before it is hidden in location B for another retrieval. A third, combined set-up was used by Amici et al. (2008). They hid the target in location A three times, then in the fourth trial they visibly moved it to location B after first hiding it at A. Zucca et al. (2007) repeated the hiding in location A twice before visibly hiding the target in location B. Most other current studies omit the repeating stage, such as Deppe et al. (2009) in their study with lemurs. These rather substantial differences in methodology need to be taken into account when considering and comparing findings. The method without the repeated retrieval excludes the explanation that the error is caused by motor memory. But it needs to be studied further whether errors in the visible displacement task with no repetition involve the same cognitive processes as the A-not-B error as initially investigated. Some authors suggest that social factors could explain the preference for the A location in certain species (Topál et al. 2008). Because the experimenter points to the location, interacts with it, and thereby teaches the animal where to look, it tends to focus on this location, instead of searching for the target independently. As seen in the history of animal cognition (Clever Hans – Pfungst 1911), every human involvement in an experiment can affect the outcome, especially in domesticated species such as dogs. This needs to be taken into account when designing and interpreting experiments, even if it does not apply to all species. Kis et al. (2012), for example, found that the “teaching” factor does not apply to common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus).

Table 1 provides a sample of studies across a range of species. For an overview of recent studies, see Cacchione and Rakoczy (2017).
Table 1

Selected examples of studies reporting performance on successive visible displacement tasks

Species

 

References

Show the A-not-B error?

Grey parrot

Psittacus erithacus

Pepperberg et al. 1997

Yes

Yellow crowned parakeet

Cyanoramphus auriceps

Funk and Matteson 2004

Yes

Cat

Felis catus

Dumas and Doré 1989

Yes

Kitten

Felis catus

Dumas and Dore 1991

No

Wolf

Canis lupus lupus

Fiset and Plourde 2013

No

Puppy

Canis lupus familiaris

Gagnon and Doré 1994

No

Goat

Capra aegagrus hircus

Nawroth et al. 2015

No

Carrion crow

Corvus corone

Hoffmann et al. 2011

Yes

Eurasian jay

Garrulus glandarius

Zucca et al. 2007

Yes

Gorilla

Gorilla gorilla

Amici et al. 2008

No

Chimpanzee

Pan troglodytes

Amici et al. 2008

No

Bonobo

Pan paniscus

Amici et al. 2008

No

Orang-utan

Pongo pygmaeus

Amici et al. 2008

Yes

Capuchin monkey

Cebus apella

Amici et al. 2008

Yes

Long-tailed macaque

Macaca fascicularis

Amici et al. 2008

Yes

Many species tested show the A-not-B error during their early development, but not when they are adults. Their developmental cognitive changes follow the same path as those of humans. The only exception so far are domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) and cats (Felis catus). Neither puppies (Gagnon and Doré 1994) nor kittens show the perseveration error (Dumas and Dore 1991) whereas adult cats do (Dumas and Doré 1989). For adult dogs, the results are inconclusive as different set-ups have been used in different studies. The specific methodology of each study must be taken into account before any comparisons between species can be made.

Spatial A-Not-B Errors

The A-not-B error involves the perseveration of a movement toward one location, and the inability to switch the movement away from it, despite the fact that the target was obviously moved toward a new location. The testing set-up for infants usually requires reaching for the target. But this behavior can also been seen on a larger scale, in detour tasks. The perseveration error occurs when participants attempt to use a previous route around a barrier despite a visible change in the correct path, thus rendering the attempted route impossible (McKenzie and Bigelow 1986). The basic set-up has the participant facing a straight barrier and they have to move around it through a gap at one end toward a target on the other side. The gap to walk through is in location A. After a few repeats, the barrier is moved so that the gap is now in location B, the other end of the barrier. There is no change in the size of the gap, or the difficulty in the task, only a change in location. This spatial set-up removes the experimenter input of touching or indicating the target locations and therefore excludes social explanations for any errors. Perseveration cannot be explained by simple motor memory, as the detours take several seconds and involve far more complex movements than simple reaching, as in the original task. McKenzy and Bigelow (1986) explored this detour behavior in human infants and found the spatial equivalent of the A-not-B error in young infants (10-months old), but not in 14-month-old children. This indicates that the same cognitive development applies to both the searching and the detour behavior.

Several studies have shown that dogs will follow a previously learned path even if this no longer leads directly to the goal. Buytendijk and Fischel (1932) were the first to describe how dogs persevere with a detour path, even after changes in the set-up would make an alternative path much faster and easier. Clarke et al. (1951) found that 10-month-old puppies showed perseveration errors in detour tasks. Puppies persisted in following the wrong, but previously learned paths even if this resulted in falling from the end of a false ramp in an elevated detour test. These findings were confirmed for adult dogs reared in restricted environments by Thompson (1954).

More recently, the spatial A-not-B error has been explored in dogs (Osthaus et al. 2010) as well as horses, mules, and donkeys (Osthaus et al. 2013). The number of repetitions of the A trial varied and therefore allowed an exploration of the influence of repetition on perseveration. Unlike in the search set-up, adult dogs persevere in the detour task, even after one A trial. Horses perform comparably, whereas mules and donkeys had lower rates of spatial perseverance and showed fewer A-not-B errors. So far, equines have not yet been tested for A-not-B errors involving the search set-up. Further tests show the same error in cats, chickens, and sheep (Osthaus, unpublished data).

Summary

The A-not-B reaching error has been shown to exist in most nonhuman species tested so far (Table 1). There are a few exceptions: apes (apart from orangutans), puppies, kittens, wolves, and goats. It must be taken into account that, although there is growing number of studies investigating object permanence in a wide range of species, most of them do not use specific tests to investigate the A-not-B error. In addition, the methodologies vary immensely between studies. Although many studies use the basic set of tests by Uzgiris and Hunt (1989) that explore successive visible displacements, the precise set-up varies between studies and makes comparisons near impossible. It is therefore difficult to compare species with regard to the A-not-B error until a standard test with a defined number of repetitions and movements between targets is implemented.

Conclusion

The A-not-B error is a behavior observed across the animal kingdom and its causes remain undiscovered to date. Only with standardized tests will it be possible to investigate and identify the underlying cognitive processes.

Cross-References

References

  1. Amici, F., Aureli, F., & Call, J. (2008). Fission-fusion dynamics, behavioral flexibility, and inhibitory control in primates. Current Biology, 18(18), 1415–1419.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Buytendijk, F. J. J., & Fischel, W. (1932). Die Bedeutung der Feldkraefte und der Intentionalitaet fuer das Verhalten des Hundes. Archives Neerlandaises de Physiologie de l’homme et des animaux, 17, 459–494.Google Scholar
  3. Cacchione, T., & Rakoczy, H. (2017). Comparative metaphysics: Thinking about objects in space and time. In J. Call (Ed.), APA handbook of comparative psychology (pp. 579–599). Washington, DC: APA Press.Google Scholar
  4. Clarke, R. S., Heron, W., Fetherstonhaugh, M. L., Forgays, D. G., & Hebb, D. O. (1951). Individual differences in dogs: Preliminary report on the effects of early experience. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 5(4), 150–156.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Deppe, A. M., Wright, P. C., & Szelistowski, W. A. (2009). Object permanence in lemurs. Animal Cognition, 12(2), 381–388.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Diamond, A. (1990). The development and neural bases of memory functions as indexed by the AB and delayed response tasks in human infants and infant monkeys. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 608(1), 267–317.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Dumas, C., & Doré, F. Y. (1989). Cognitive development in kittens (Felis catus): A cross-sectional study of object permanence. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 103, 191–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Dumas, C., & Dore, F. Y. (1991). Cognitive development in kittens (Felis catus): An observational study of object permanence and sensorimotor intelligence. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 105, 357–365.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Etienne, A. S. (1973). Searching behavior towards a disappearing prey in the domestic chick as affected by preliminary experience. Animal Behavior, 21, 749–761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Fiset, S., & Plourde, V. (2013). Object permanence in domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) and gray wolves (Canis lupus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 127(2), 115.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Funk, M. S., & Matteson, R. L. (2004). Stable individual differences on developmental tasks in young yellow-crowned parakeets, Cyanoramphus auriceps. Animal Learning & Behavior, 32(4), 427–439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Gagnon, S., & Doré, F. Y. (1994). Cross-sectional study of object permanence in domestic puppies (Canis familiaris). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108(3), 220.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Gruber, H. E., Girgus, J. S., & Banuazizi, A. (1971). The development of object permanence in the cat. Developmental Psychology, 4, 9–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hoffmann, A., Rüttler, V., & Nieder, A. (2011). Ontogeny of object permanence and object tracking in the carrion crow, Corvus corone. Animal Behaviour, 82(2), 359–367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Kis, A., Gácsi, M., Range, F., & Virányi, Z. (2012). Object permanence in adult common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus): Not everything is an “A-not-B” error that seems to be one. Animal Cognition, 15(1), 97–105.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Markovitch, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (1999). The A-not-B error: Results from a logistic meta-analysis. Child Development, 70, 1297–1313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. McKenzie, B. E., & Bigelow, E. (1986). Detour behavior in young human infants. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4(2), 139–148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Nawroth, C., von Borell, E., & Langbein, J. (2015). Object permanence in the dwarf goat (Capra aegagrus hircus): Perseveration errors and the tracking of complex movements of hidden objects. Applied Animal Behavior Science, 167, 20–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Osthaus, B., Marlow, D., & Ducat, P. (2010). Minding the gap: Spatial perseveration error in dogs. Animal Cognition, 13(6), 881–885.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Osthaus, B., Proops, L., Hocking, I., & Burden, F. (2013). Spatial cognition and perseveration by horses, donkeys and mules in a simple A-not-B detour task. Animal Cognition, 16(2), 301–305.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Pepperberg, I. M. (2002). The value of the Piagetian framework for comparative cognitive studies. Animal Cognition, 5, 177–182.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Pepperberg, I. M., Willner, M. R., & Gravitz, L. B. (1997). Development of Piagetian object permanence in grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 111(1), 63.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Pfungst, O. (1911). Clever Hans: (The horse of Mr. Von Osten.) a contribution to experimental animal and human psychology. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child (M. Cook, Trans.). New York: Basic Books. (Original work published 1937).Google Scholar
  25. Piaget, J. (1971). Biology and knowledge. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  26. Pollok, B., Prior, H., & Güntürkün, O. (2000). Development of object permanence in food-storing magpies (Pica pica). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 114(2), 148–157.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Thinus-Blanc, C., & Scardigli, P. (1981). Object permanence in the golden hamster. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 53, 1010.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Thompson, W. R. (1954). The effects of restricting early experience on the problem-solving capacity of dogs. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 8, 17–31.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Topál, J., Gergely, G., Miklósi, Á., Erdőhegyi, Á., & Csibra, G. (2008). Infants’ perseverative search errors are induced by pragmatic misinterpretation. Science, 321(5897), 1831–1834.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Uzgiris, I. C., & Hunt, J. M. (1989). Assessment in infancy: Ordinal scales of psychological development (2nd ed.). Urbana: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
  31. Vauclair, J. (1996). Animal cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  32. Zucca, P., Milos, N., & Vallortigara, G. (2007). Piagetian object permanence and its development in Eurasian jays (Garrulus glandarius). Animal Cognition, 10(2), 243–258.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of Psychology, Politics and SociologyCanterbury Christ Church UniversityCanterburyUK

Section editors and affiliations

  • Mark A. Krause
    • 1
  1. 1.Southern Oregon UniversityAshlandUSA