Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences

Living Edition
| Editors: Virgil Zeigler-Hill, Todd K. Shackelford

Personal Agency

  • Sinan AlperEmail author
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-28099-8_1871-1

Synonyms

Definition

Personal agency refers to “the sense that I am the one who is causing or generating an action” (Gallagher 2000, p. 15). A person with a sense of personal agency perceives himself/herself as the subject influencing his/her own actions and life circumstances (Bandura 2006; Gallagher 2000).

Introduction

Compared to other animals, humans are better at resisting the influence of external forces and independently generating their own actions. Having been one of the central issues in philosophy for centuries, the personal agency of humans has also become a matter of interest in psychological research.

Properties of Agency

According to social cognitive theory approach, personal agency is comprised of four properties: Intentionality, forethought, self-reactiveness, and self-reflectiveness (Bandura 2006). The sense of agency requires (1) intentional planning of actions; (2) predicting likely outcomes of actions; (3) regulating own behavior to reach goals; and (4) examining own performance and adjusting when necessary (Bandura 2006).

Perceived Self-Efficacy

The sense of agency is closely related to the concept of self-efficacy which refers to the belief of being capable of accomplishing a goal (Bandura 2006). People with high perceived self-efficacy have a strong sense of agency as they perceive themselves as the subject shaping the course of events in their lives. Past research suggested that high self-efficacy was related to increased motivation, performance, and resilience (Bandura 2000).

Development of Sense of Agency

Children are not born with a sense of agency. Starting with the first months, infants begin realizing their control over their own bodies and the environment (Mandler 1992). By perceiving the causal relationships behind actions that were directed at them as well as their own actions, infants slowly develop a sense of agency (Bandura 2006; Haggard 2017; Mandler 1992). With the acquisition of language, having been referred to by personal names, children further differentiate themselves as separate self-governing individuals (Lewis and Brooks-Gunn 1979).

Neural Correlates of Agency

Recent neuropsychological research has examined potential brain mechanisms underlying the sense of agency. A meta-analysis suggested that temporoparietal junction area in the brain is related to “non-agency,” or in other words, the perception of not being the agent behind an effect (Haggard 2017). Recent findings also suggested that the connectivity between frontal and prefrontal motor areas might have a key role in the production of the sense of agency (Haggard 2017).

Models of Agency

Individuals differ in their way of obtaining a sense of agency. The disjoint agency, which is characteristic of independent cultures, is based on sustaining personal control over the events in life and protecting oneself against the influence of external forces (Markus and Kitayama 2003). The conjoint agency, which is characteristic of interdependent cultures, is built around the emphasis on maintaining the harmony within the ingroup and feeling a part of a greater whole (Markus and Kitayama 2003). People can also exercise a proxy agency which refers to reaching goals by influencing others who have access to the resources needed (Bandura 2006).

Lack of Sense of Agency

Lacking a normal level of sense of agency can lead to adverse consequences (Haggard 2017). Reduced sense of agency is a common symptom observed in patients suffering from depression who lack a sense of perceived control over the events in their lives (Harrow et al. 2009). Schizophrenia patients similarly experience a loss of sense of agency over their actions (Maeda et al. 2013). People with low self-efficacy are also less likely to maintain desired health behaviors (Schwarzer and Renner 2000).

Conclusion

Having a sense of agency is desired for most individuals, and past literature illustrated many examples of how it is related to desired outcomes. People with high self-efficacy are better at reaching their goals and have better mental as well as physical health. Although individuals and cultures might differ regarding what type of agency is more valued, a general sense of agency is still considered a fundamental psychological need.

Cross-References

References

  1. Bandura, A. (2000). Exercise of human agency through collective efficacy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 75–78.  https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00064.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bandura, A. (2006). Toward a psychology of human agency. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 164–180.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00011.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Gallagher, S. (2000). Philosophical conceptions of the self: Implications for cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 14–21.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01417-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Haggard, P. (2017). Sense of agency in the human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18, 196–207.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2017.14.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Harrow, M., Hansford, B. G., & Astrachan-Fletcher, E. B. (2009). Locus of control: Relation to schizophrenia, to recovery, and to depression and psychosis – A 15-year longitudinal study. Psychiatry Research, 168, 186–192.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2008.06.002.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  6. Lewis, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Toward a theory of social cognition: The development of self. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 1979, 1–20.  https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.23219790403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Maeda, T., Takahata, K., Muramatsu, T., Okimura, T., Koreki, A., Iwashita, S., Mimura, M., & Kato, M. (2013). Reduced sense of agency in chronic schizophrenia with predominant negative symptoms. Psychiatry Research, 209, 386–392.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.04.017.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Mandler, J. M. (1992). How to build a baby: II. Conceptual primitives. Psychological Review, 99, 587–604.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (2003). Models of agency: Sociocultural diversity in the construction of action. In V. M. Berman & J. J. Berman (Eds.), Nebraska symposium on motivation: Cross-cultural differences in perspectives on the self (Vol. 49, pp. 1–57). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  10. Schwarzer, R., & Renner, B. (2000). Social-cognitive predictors of health behavior: Action self-efficacy and coping self-efficacy. Health Psychology, 19, 487–495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyYasar UniversityIzmirTurkey

Section editors and affiliations

  • John F. Rauthmann
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversität zu LübeckLübeckGermany