Skip to main content

Obedience

  • Living reference work entry
  • First Online:
  • 117 Accesses

Definition

Submission to the requirements of an authority.

Introduction

The standard definition of obedience in psychology is as a form of social influence elicited in response to a direct order or command. However, there are reasons for suggesting that this definition is too narrow in that it specifies that a particular social act – the order or command – is necessary for obedience to occur. Consideration of how the term “obedience” and its derivatives are used in everyday language is instructive. People refer to obeying the law or obeying the word of God, but in neither situation is a direct order required. For example, we don’t typically need an authority figure such as a police officer to order us to drive on the correct side of road or to refrain from leaving a store without paying for our goods. Indeed, for a society to function smoothly, it is necessary that people are able to go about their daily lives without authority having to be exercised directly in the form of explicit...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

References

  • Baumrind, D. (1964). Some thoughts on ethics of research: After reading Milgram’s “Behavioural study of obedience”. American Psychologist, 19, 421–423. doi:10.1037/h0040128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beauvois, J.-L., Courbet, D., & Oberlé, D. (2012). The prescriptive power of the television host. A transportation of Milgram’s obedience paradigm to the context of TV game show. European Review of Applied Psychology, 62, 111–119. doi:10.1016/j.erap.2012.02.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bègue, L., Beauvois, J.-L., Courbet, D., Oberlé, D., Lepage, J., & Duke, A. A. (2015). Personality predicts obedience in a Milgram paradigm. Journal of Personality, 83, 299–306. doi:10.1111/jopy.12104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blass, T. (2004). The man who shocked the world: The life and legacy of Stanley Milgram. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blass, T. (2012). A cross-cultural comparison of studies of obedience using the Milgram paradigm: A review. Social & Personality Psychology Compass, 6, 196–205. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00417.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bocchiaro, P., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2010). Defying unjust authority: An exploratory study. Current Psychology, 29, 155–170. doi:10.1007/s12144-010-9080-z.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Burger, J. M. (2009). Replicating Milgram: Would people still obey today? American Psychologist, 64, 1–11. doi:10.1037/a0010932.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burger, J. M., Girgis, Z. M., & Manning, C. C. (2011). In their own words: Explaining obedience to authority through an analysis of participants’ comments. Social Psychological & Personality Science, 2, 460–466. doi:10.1177/1948550610397632.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dambrun, M., & Vatiné, E. (2010). Reopening the study of extreme behaviors: Obedience to authority within an immersive video environment. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 760–773. doi:10.1002/ejsp.646.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elms, A. C., & Milgram, S. (1966). Personality characteristics associated with obedience and defiance toward authoritative command. Journal of Experimental Research in Personality, 1, 282–289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foucault, M. (1979). Discipline and punish: The birth of the prison. London: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, S. (2013a). ‘The last possible resort’: A forgotten prod and the in situ standardization of Stanley Milgram’s voice-feedback condition. History of Psychology, 16, 177–194. doi:10.1037/a0032430.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, S. (2013b). Milgram’s obedience experiments: A rhetorical analysis. British Journal of Social Psychology, 52, 290–309. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8309.2011.02070.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haslam, S. A., Reicher, S. D., & Birney, M. E. (2014). Nothing by mere authority: Evidence that in an experimental analogue of the Milgram paradigm participants are motivated not by orders but by appeals to authority. Journal of Social Issues, 70, 473–488. doi:10.1111/josi.12072.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haslam, S. A., Reicher, S. D., & Millard, K. (2015). Shock treatment: Using immersive digital realism to restage and re-examine Milgram’s ‘obedience to authority’ research. PLoS ONE, 10(3), e109015. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0109015.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to authority: An experimental view. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Millard, K. (2014). Revisioning obedience: Exploring the role of Milgram’s skills as a filmmaker in bringing his shocking narrative to life. Journal of Social Issues, 70, 439–455. doi:10.1111/josi.12070.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, A. G. (1986). The obedience experiments: A case study of controversy in social science. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson, I. (2011). Torture at Yale”: Experimental subjects, laboratory torment and the “rehabilitation” of Milgram’s “obedience to authority”. Theory & Psychology, 21, 737–761. doi:10.1177/0959354311420199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orne, M. T., & Holland, C. C. (1968). On the ecological validity of laboratory deceptions. International Journal of Psychiatry, 6, 282–293.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perry, G. (2012). Behind the shock machine: The untold story of the notorious Milgram psychology experiments. London: Scribe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reicher, S., & Haslam, S. A. (2011). After shock? Towards a social identity explanation of the Milgram ‘obedience’ studies. British Journal of Social Psychology, 50, 163–169. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8309.2010.02015.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, N. J. C. (2011). Milgram’s obedience to authority experiments: Origins and early evolution. British Journal of Social Psychology, 50, 140–162. doi:10.1348/014466610X492205.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. R. (1975). Indirect speech acts. In P. Cole & J. L. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics. volume 3: Speech acts (pp. 59–82). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slater, M., Antley, A., Davison, A., Swapp, D., Guger, C., Barker, C., Pistrang, N., & Sanchez-Vives, M. V. (2006). A virtual reprise of the Stanley Milgram obedience experiments. PLoS ONE, 1(1), e39. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000039.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zeigler-Hill, V., Southard, A. C., Archer, L. M., & Donohoe, P. L. (2013). Neuroticism and negative affect influence the reluctance to engage in destructive obedience in the Milgram paradigm. The Journal of Social Psychology, 153, 161–174. doi:10.1080/00224545.2012.713041.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stephen Gibson .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Section Editor information

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing AG

About this entry

Cite this entry

Gibson, S. (2016). Obedience. In: Zeigler-Hill, V., Shackelford, T. (eds) Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-28099-8_1257-1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-28099-8_1257-1

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-28099-8

  • eBook Packages: Springer Reference Behavioral Science and PsychologyReference Module Humanities and Social SciencesReference Module Business, Economics and Social Sciences

Publish with us

Policies and ethics