Collectivistic Cultures
- 68 Downloads
Definition
Among the variations between cultures, one prominent difference regards the relative emphasis of personal desires versus the goals of the group to which the individual belongs to. In collectivistic cultures, individuals view themselves primarily as parts of a larger whole (e.g., family) and are motivated mainly by norms and obligations imposed by the collective entity (Triandis 1995). Collectivistic cultures are further distinguished by the horizontal versus vertical dimension. In horizontal collectivistic cultures, a sense of social cohesion and oneness with in-group members is emphasized, whereas vertical collectivistic cultures expect sacrifice for the in-group and acceptance of inequalities within the collective (Singelis et al. 1995).
Introduction
Although cultural syndromes organized around the theme of collective units versus the individual person have been discussed for centuries (e.g., gemeinschaft versus gesellschaftamong German scholars), since the early 1990s,...
References
- Berry, J. W. (1979). A cultural ecology of social behavior. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 12, pp. 177–207). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
- Chiao, J. Y., & Blizinsky, K. D. (2010). Culture-gene coevolution of individualism-collectivism and the serotonin transporter gene. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 277, 529–537.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Heine, S. J., Kitayama, S., Lehman, D. R., Takata, T., Ide, E., Leung, C., & Matsumoto, H. (2001). Divergent consequences of success and failure in Japan and North America: An investigation of self-improving motivation and malleable selves. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 599–615.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Hofstede, G. (1991). Cultures and organizations. London: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
- Markus, H., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (1999). Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. American Psychologist, 54, 741–754.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Singelis, T. M., Triandis, H. C., Bhawuk, D. P., & Gelfand, M. J. (1995). Horizontal and vertical dimensions of individualism and collectivism: A theoretical and measurement refinement. Cross-Cultural Research, 29, 240–275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Suh, E., Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Triandis, H. C. (1998). The shifting basis of life satisfaction judgments across cultures: Emotions versus norms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 482–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Talhelm, T., Zhang, X., Oishi, S., Shimin, C., Duan, D., Lan, X., & Kitayama, S. (2014). Large-scale psychological differences within China explained by rice versus wheat agriculture. Science, 344, 603–608.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Triandis, H. C. (1994). Culture and social behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
- Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Westview: Boulder.Google Scholar
- Triandis, H. C. (1990). Cross-cultural studies of individualism and collectivism. In J. Berman (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation, 1989 (pp. 41–133). University of Nebraska Press: Lincoln.Google Scholar