Alexander, R. D. (1989). Evolution of the human psyche. In P. Mellars & C. Stringer (Eds.), The human revolution: Behavioural and biological perspectives on the origins of modern humans (pp. 455–513). Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417–423.
CrossRef
PubMed
Google Scholar
Barrett, H. C., & Kurzban, R. (2006). Modularity in cognition: Framing the debate. Psychological Review, 113, 628–647.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Carew, T. J., Walters, E. T., & Kandel, E. R. (1981). Classical conditioning in a simple withdrawal reflex in Aplysia californica. The Journal of Neuroscience, 1, 1426–1437.
PubMed
PubMed Central
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Carpenter, P., Just, M., & Shell, P. (1990). What one intelligence test measures: A theoretical account of the processing in the Raven Progressive Matrices Test. Psychological Review, 97, 404–431.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Case, R., Demetriou, A., Platsidou, M., & Kazi, S. (2001). Integrating concepts and tests of intelligence from the differential and developmental traditions. Intelligence, 29, 307–336.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Chiappe, D., & MacDonald, K. B. (2005). The evolution of domain-general mechanisms in intelligence and learning. Journal of General Psychology, 132, 5–40.
CrossRef
PubMed
Google Scholar
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2002). Unraveling the enigma of human intelligence: Evolutionary psychology and the multimodular mind. In R. J. Sternberg & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), The evolution of intelligence (pp. 145–198). Mahwah: Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Crinella, F. M., & Yu, J. (1995). Brain mechanisms in problem solving and intelligence: A replication and extension. Intelligence, 21, 225–246.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Figueredo, A. J., Brumbach, B. H., & Schlomer, G. L. (2009). Fundamental dimensions of environmental risk: The impact of harsh versus unpredictable environments on the evolution and development of life history strategies. Human Nature, 20, 204–298.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Fernandes, H. B. F., Woodley, M. A., & te Nijenhuis, J. (2014). Differences in cognitive abilities among primates are concentrated on G: Phenotypic and phylogenetic comparisons with two meta-analytical databases. Intelligence, 46, 311–322.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Galsworthy, M. J., Arden, R., & Chabris, C. F. (2014). Animal models of general cognitive ability for genetic research into cognitive functioning. In D. Finkel & C. A. Reynolds (Eds.), Behavior genetics of cognition across the lifespan (pp. 257–278). New York: Springer.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (2005). The origin of mind: Evolution of brain, cognition, and general intelligence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (2015). Social competition and the evolution of fluid intelligence. In S. Goldstein, D. Princiotta, & J. A. Naglieri (Eds.), Handbook of intelligence: Evolutionary theory, historical perspective, and current concepts (pp. 105–119). New York: Springer.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Heinrich, B. (2000). Testing insight in ravens. In C. Heyes & L. Huber (Eds.), The evolution of cognition (pp. 289–305). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Jensen, A. (1998). The g factor: The science of mental ability. Westport: Praeger.
Google Scholar
Jerison, H. J. (1973). The evolution of the brain and intelligence. New York: Academic.
Google Scholar
Kane, M. J., Bleckley, M. K., Conway, A. R., & Engle, R. (2001). A controlled-attention view of working memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 169–183.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Kaplan, H., Hill, K., Lancaster, J., & Hurtado, A. M. (2000). A theory of human life history evolution: Diet, intelligence, and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9, 156–185.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Kyllonen, P. C., & Christal, R. E. (1990). Reasoning ability is (little more than) working memory capacity?! Intelligence, 14, 389–433.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Larson, G., & Saccuzzo, D. (1989). Cognitive correlates of general intelligence: Toward a process theory of g. Intelligence, 13, 5–31.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Lefebvre, L., Whittle, P., Lascaris, E., & Finkelstein, A. (1997). Feeding innovations and forebrain size in birds. Animal Behaviour, 53, 549–560.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Lynn, R. (2006). Race differences in intelligence: An evolutionary analysis. Augusta: Washington Summit Press.
Google Scholar
MacDonald, K. B. (2008). Effortful control, explicit processing and the regulation of human evolved predispositions. Psychological Review, 115, 1012–1031.
PubMed
CrossRef
Google Scholar
MacDonald, K. B. (2013). Human general intelligence as a domain general psychological adaptation. In J. Kush (Ed.), Intelligence quotient: Testing, role of genetics and the environment and social outcomes (pp. 35–54). Hauppauge: Nova Science Publishers.
Google Scholar
Potts, R. (1998). Variability selection in hominid evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 7, 81–96.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Reader, S. M., & Laland, K. N. (2002). Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 99, 4436–4441.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Reader, S. M., Hager, Y., & Laland, K. N. (2011). The evolution of primate general and cultural intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences, 366, 1017–1027.
CrossRef
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Rushton, J. P. (2004). Placing intelligence into an evolutionary framework, or how g fits into the r-K matrix of life history traits including longevity. Intelligence, 32, 321–328.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Rushton, J. P., & Jensen, A. R. (2010). The rise and fall of the Flynn effect as a reason to expect the narrowing of the Black–White gap. Intelligence, 38, 213–219.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (2004). The robot’s rebellion: Finding meaning in the age of Darwin. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Stevenson, H., Parker, T., Wilkinson, A., Hegion, A., & Fish, E. (1976). Longitudinal study of individual differences in cognitive development and scholastic achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 68, 377–400.
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Taylor, A. H., Elliffe, D. M., Hunt, G. R., Emery, N. J., Clayton, N. S., & Gray, R. D. (2011). New caledonian crows learn the functional properties of novel tool types. PLoS ONE, 6(12), e26887.
PubMed
PubMed Central
CrossRef
Google Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 19–136). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Vrba, E. S. (1995). The fossil record of African antelopes (Mammalia, Bovidae) in relation to human evolution and paleoclimate. In E. S. Vrba, G. H. Denton, T. C. Partridge, & L. H. Burckle (Eds.), Paleoclimate and evolution, with emphasis on human origins (pp. 385–424). New Haven: Yale University Press.
Google Scholar
Woodley of Menie, M. A., Fernandes, H. B. F., & Hopkins, W. D. (2015). The more g-loaded, the more heritable, evolvable, and phenotypically variable: Homology with humans in chimpanzee cognitive abilities. Intelligence, 50, 159–163.
PubMed
PubMed Central
CrossRef
Google Scholar