Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science

Living Edition
| Editors: Todd K. Shackelford, Viviana A. Weekes-Shackelford

Psychology of Reciprocal Altruism

  • Jeffrey R. Stevens
  • Juan F. Duque
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_3051-1

Synonyms

Definition

Different forms of reciprocal altruism – taking turns offering altruistic help – require different psychological building blocks.

Introduction

A pair of pied flycatchers busily snatch insects to bring back to their nest and feed to their young. Suddenly, an owl appears close to the nest, posing a serious predation threat to their offspring. The pair fly straight to the owl, shrieking and dive bombing it to chase it away. A nearby pair of flycatchers detects the ruckus and also arrives to attack the predator. Another nearby pair, however, fails to help. An hour later, two owls appear, one at each of the neighboring nests. What should the original pair do? Krams et al. (2008) conducted this experiment and found that, out of 32 pairs of flycatchers, 2 stayed at the nest, 30 attacked the owl near the pair who helped them previously, and none attacked the owl near the pair who did not help them before. This willingness to pay a cost to cooperate or help...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Aktipis, C. A. (2006). Recognition memory and the evolution of cooperation: How simple strategies succeed in an agent-based world. Adaptive Behavior, 14(3), 239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Axelrod, R., & Hamilton, W. D. (1981). The evolution of cooperation. Science, 211(4489), 1390–1396.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Barclay, P. (2008). Enhanced recognition of defectors depends on their rarity. Cognition, 107(3), 817–828.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Basu, S., Dickhaut, J., Hecht, G., Towry, K., & Waymire, G. (2009). Recordkeeping alters economic history by promoting reciprocity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 106(4), 1009–1014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bell, R., & Buchner, A. (2012). How adaptive is memory for cheaters? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(6), 403–408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Boysen, S. T., & Berntson, G. G. (1995). Responses to quantity: Perceptual versus cognitive mechanisms in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 21(1), 82–86.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Brosnan, S. F., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). A proximate perspective on reciprocal altruism. Human Nature, 13(1), 129–152.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1989). Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture: II. Case study: A computational theory of social exchange. Ethology and Sociobiology, 10(1–3), 51–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. de Waal, F. B. M., & Luttrell, L. M. (1988). Mechanisms of social reciprocity in three primate species: Symmetrical relationship characteristics or cognition? Ethology and Sociobiology, 9(2), 101–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Dugatkin, L. A. (1997). Cooperation among animals: An evolutionary perspective. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  11. Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S., & Spelke, E. S. (2004). Core systems of number. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(7), 307–314.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Fessler, D. M. T., & Haley, K. J. (2003). The strategy of affect: Emotions in human cooperation. In P. Hammerstein (Ed.), Genetic and cultural evolution of cooperation, Dahlem Workshop Report (pp. 7–36). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  13. Frederick, S., Loewenstein, G., & O’Donoghue, T. (2002). Time discounting and time preference: A critical review. Journal of Economic Literature, 40(2), 351–401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Furlong, E. E., & Opfer, J. E. (2009). Cognitive constraints on how economic rewards affect cooperation. Psychological Science, 20(1), 11–16.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I, II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 1–52.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Harris, A. C., & Madden, G. J. (2002). Delay discounting and performance on the prisoner’s dilemma game. Psychological Record, 52(4), 429–440.Google Scholar
  17. Hayden, B. Y. (2015). Time discounting and time preference in animals: A critical review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(1), 39–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Krams, I., Krama, T., Igaune, K., & Mänd, R. (2008). Experimental evidence of reciprocal altruism in the pied flycatcher. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 62(4), 599–605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Kugler, K., & Jones, W. H. (1992). On conceptualizing and assessing guilt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(2), 318–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. McCullough, M. E., Kilpatrick, S. D., Emmons, R. A., & Larson, D. B. (2001). Is gratitude a moral affect? Psychological Bulletin, 127(2), 249–266.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Nissen, H. J., Damerow, P., & Englund, R. K. (1993). Archaic bookkeeping. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  22. Nowak, M. A., & Sigmund, K. (1998). The dynamics of indirect reciprocity. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 194(4), 561–574.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Pfeiffer, T., Rutte, C., Killingback, T., Taborsky, M., & Bonhoeffer, S. (2005). Evolution of cooperation by generalized reciprocity. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B, 272(1568), 1115–1120.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  24. Schino, G., & Aureli, F. (2010). Primate reciprocity and its cognitive requirements. Evolutionary Anthropology, 19(4), 130–135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (1984). Grooming, alliances and reciprocal altruism in vervet monkeys. Nature, 308(5959), 541–543.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Shackelford, T. K., & Buss, D. M. (1996). Betrayal in mateships, friendships, and coalitions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(11), 1151–1164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Shifferman, E. (2009). Its own reward: Lessons to be drawn from the reversed-reward contingency paradigm. Animal Cognition, 12(4), 547–558.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Sommerfeld, R. D., Krambeck, H.-J., Semmann, D., & Milinski, M. (2007). Gossip as an alternative for direct observation in games of indirect reciprocity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 104(44), 17435–17440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Stephens, D. W., McLinn, C. M., & Stevens, J. R. (2002). Discounting and reciprocity in an Iterated Prisoner’s Dilemma. Science, 298(5601), 2216–2218.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Stevens, J. R., Volstorf, J., Schooler, L. J., & Rieskamp, J. (2011). Forgetting constrains the emergence of cooperative decision strategies. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 235.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  31. Tibbetts, E. A., & Dale, J. (2007). Individual recognition: It is good to be different. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 22(10), 529–537.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46(1), 35–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of Nebraska-LincolnLincolnUSA