Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science

Living Edition
| Editors: Todd K. Shackelford, Viviana A. Weekes-Shackelford

Disease Avoidance

  • Justin H. ParkEmail author
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_2971-1

Synonyms

Definition

Behavioral disease-avoidance processes help animals maintain safe distances from sources of contagious disease, thereby reducing the need for physiological immune responses.

Introduction

Threats to survival have been a major driving force in evolution, shaping a wide range of specialized defensive adaptations. Some of the threats come in the form of the many hazards in the physical environment. Other, perhaps more potent threats come in the form of exploitation by other organisms. Predators are an obvious example, as are other members of the species competing for limited resources. Although less conspicuous than typical predators, tiny organisms that infiltrate larger organisms and cause somatic damage have posed a recurrent and pernicious threat. These tiny organisms include microbes such as viruses, bacteria, and protozoa, as well as relatively larger organisms such...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Case, T. I., Repacholi, B. M., & Stevenson, R. J. (2006). My baby doesn’t smell as bad as yours: The plasticity of disgust. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 357–365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Clark, W. R. (2008). defense of self: How the immune system really works. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Clark, J. A., & Fessler, D. M. T. (2014). Recontextualizing the behavioral immune system within psychoneuroimmunology. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 8, 235–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Curtis, V. (2013). Don’t look, don’t touch: The science behind revulsion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Darwin, C. R. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London: John Murray.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Duncan, L. A., Schaller, M., & Park, J. H. (2009). Perceived vulnerability to disease: Development and validation of a 15-item self-report instrument. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 541–546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Faulkner, J., Schaller, M., Park, J. H., & Duncan, L. A. (2004). Evolved disease-avoidance mechanisms and contemporary xenophobic attitudes. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 7, 333–353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Gangestad, S. W., & Buss, D. M. (1993). Pathogen prevalence and human mate preferences. Ethology and Sociobiology, 14, 89–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Gangestad, S. W., & Grebe, N. M. (2014). Pathogen avoidance within an integrated immune system: Multiple components with distinct costs and benefits. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 8, 226–234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Gelfand, M. J., Raver, J. L., Nishii, L., Leslie, L. M., Lun, J., Lim, B. C., et al. (2011). Differences between tight and loose cultures: A 33-nation study. Science, 332, 1100–1104.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Goodall, J. (1986). Social rejection, exclusion, and shunning among the Gombe chimpanzees. Ethology and Sociobiology, 7, 227–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Hart, B. L. (2011). Behavioural defences in animals against pathogens and parasites: Parallels with the pillars of medicine in humans. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 3406–3417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Haselton, M. G., & Nettle, D. (2006). The paranoid optimist: An integrative evolutionary model of cognitive biases. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 47–66.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Kavaliers, M., Choleris, E., & Pfaff, D. W. (2005). Recognition and avoidance of the odors of parasitized conspecifics and predators: Differential genomic correlates. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 29, 1347–1359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Kurzban, R., & Leary, M. R. (2001). Evolutionary origins of stigmatization: The functions of social exclusion. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 187–208.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Miller, S. L., & Maner, J. K. (2012). Overperceiving disease cues: The basic cognition of the behavioral immune system. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 1198–1213.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Mortensen, C. R., Becker, D. V., Ackerman, J. M., Neuberg, S. L., & Kenrick, D. T. (2010). Infection breeds reticence: The effects of disease salience on self-perceptions of personality and behavioral avoidance tendencies. Psychological Science, 21, 440–447.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Murray, D. R., & Schaller, M. (2016). The behavioral immune system: Implications for social cognition, social interaction, and social influence. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 53, 75–129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Navarrete, C. D., & Fessler, D. M. T. (2006). Disease avoidance and ethnocentrism: The effects of disease vulnerability and disgust sensitivity on intergroup attitudes. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 270–282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2009). Disgust as a disease-avoidance mechanism. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 303–321.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2011). Disease avoidance as a functional basis for stigmatization. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 3433–3452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Park, J. H., & Isherwood, E. (2011). Effects of concerns about pathogens on conservatism and anti-fat prejudice: Are they mediated by moral intuitions? Journal of Social Psychology, 151, 391–394.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Park, J. H., Van Leeuwen, F., & Stephen, I. D. (2012). Homeliness is in the disgust sensitivity of the beholder: Relatively unattractive faces appear especially unattractive to individuals higher in pathogen disgust. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 569–577.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  25. Pollet, T. V., Tybur, J. M., Frankenhuis, W. E., & Rickard, I. J. (2014). What can cross-cultural correlations teach us about human nature? Human Nature, 25, 410–429.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Ryan, S., Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2012). Facial disfigurement is treated like an infectious disease. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 639–646.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Schaller, M., & Park, J. H. (2011). The behavioral immune system (and why it matters). Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 99–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Terrizzi, J. A., Jr., Shook, N. J., & McDaniel, M. A. (2013). The behavioral immune system and social conservatism: A meta-analysis. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 99–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., Kurzban, R., & DeScioli, P. (2013). Disgust: Evolved function and structure. Psychological Review, 120, 65–84.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Van Leeuwen, F., Park, J. H., Koenig, B. L., & Graham, J. (2012). Regional variation in pathogen prevalence predicts endorsement of group-focused moral concerns. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 429–437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of BristolBristolUK

Section editors and affiliations

  • Justin H. Park
    • 1
  1. 1.University of BristolBristolUK