Skip to main content

Savanna Hypothesis, The

Synonyms

Habitat selection; Landscape aesthetics

Definition

The Savanna Hypothesis states that we retain genetically based preferences for features of high-quality African savannas where our ancestors lived when their brains and bodies evolved into their modern forms.

Introduction

Selection of a place to live is a crucial step in the lives of most animals. Selection depends on the recognition of objects, sounds, and odors to which an animal, molded by natural selection, responds as if it understood their significance for its future survival and reproductive success. Evolutionary theory suggests that the ability of a landscape to evoke positive emotional states should be positively correlated with the expected survival and reproductive success of individuals of that species in it. In other words, good habitats should evoke strong positive responses; poor habitats should evoke weak or negative responses (Orians and Heerwagen 1992). Habitat selection has served as a conceptual basis for...

Keywords

  • African Savanna
  • Tropical Savanna
  • Landscape Preference
  • Savanna Tree
  • Herbaceous Understory

These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

References

  • Appleton, J. (1975). The experience of landscape. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balling, J. D., & Falk, J. H. (1982). Development of visual preferences for natural environments. Environment and Behavior, 14, 5–28.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Bernáldez, F., Gallardo, D., & Abelló, R. P. (1987). Children’s landscape preferences: From rejection to attraction. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 7, 169–176.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, B. (1985). Human evolution (3rd ed.). New York: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coss, R. G. (2003). The role of evolved perceptual biases in art and design. In E. Voland & K. Grammer (Eds.), Evolutionary aesthetics (pp. 69–130). Heidelberg: Springer.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Coss, R. G., & Goldthwaite, R. O. (1995). The persistence of old designs for perception. In N. S. Thompson (Ed.), Perspectives in ethology, volume 11: Behavioral design (pp. 83–148). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coss, R. G., & Moore, M. (2002). Precocious knowledge of trees as antipredator refuge in preschool children: An examination of aesthetics, attributive judgments, and relic sexual dinichism. Ecological Psychology, 14, 181–222.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Heerwagen, J. H., & Orians, G. H. (1993). Humans, habitats, and aesthetics. In S. R. Kellert & E. O. Wilson (Eds.), The biophilia hypothesis (pp. 138–172). Washington, DC: Island Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henshilwood, C. S., d’Errico, F., Marean, C. W., Milo, R. G., & Yates, R. (2001). An early bone tool industry from the Middle Stone Age at Blombos Cave, South Africa: Implications for the origins of modern human behaviour, symbolism and language. Journal of Human Evolution, 41, 631–678.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herzog, T. R. (1985). A cognitive analysis of preference for waterscapes. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 5, 225–241.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Komar, V., & Melamid, A. (1997). In J. A. Wypijewski (Ed.), Painting by the numbers: Komar and Melamid’s scientific guide to art. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lohr, V. I., & Pearson-Mims, C. H. (2006). Responses to scenes with spreading, rounded, and conical tree forms. Environment and Behavior, 38, 667–668.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Lyons, E. (1983). Demographic correlates of landscape preference. Environment and Behavior, 15, 487–511.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Mellaart, J. (1968). Çatal Hüyük: A neolithic town in Anatolia. London: Thames and Hudson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nabhan, G. P. (2004). Why some like it hot. Food, genes, and cultural diversity. Washington, DC: Island Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Orians, G. H. (1980). Habitat selection. In J. S. Lockard (Ed.), The evolution of human social behavior (pp. 49–66). New York: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Orians, G. H. (2014). Snakes, sunrises and Shakespeare. How evolution shapes our loves and fears. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Orians, G. H., & Heerwagen, J. H. (1992). Evolved responses to landscapes. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind (pp. 555–579). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Repton, H. (1907). The art of landscape gardening. Boston/New York: Houghton Mifflin.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, S. (1998). What gardens mean. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shanes, E. (1979). Turner’s picturesque views of England and Wales, 1825–1838. London: Chatto & Windus.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shuttleworth, S. (1980). The use of photographs as an environmental presentation medium in landscape studies. Journal of Environmental Management, 11, 61–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sommer, R. (1997). Further cross-national studies of tree-form preference. Ecological Psychology, 9, 153–160.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Sommer, R., & Summit, J. (1996). Cross-national rankings of tree shape. Ecological Psychology, 8, 327–341.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Tuan, Y.-F. (1974). Topophilia. A study of environmental perception, attitudes, and values. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich, R. S. (1981). Natural versus urban spaces: Some psychophysiological effects. Environment and Behavior, 13, 523–556.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. E., Robertson, L. D., Daley, M., & Walton, S. A. (1995). Effects of visual cues on assessment of water quality. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 15, 53–63.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Witzel, M. (2015). Water in mythology. Daedalus, 144, 18–26.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham, R. (2009). Catching fire. How cooking made us human. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zube, E. H., Simcox, D. E., & Law, C. S. (1987). Perceptual landscape simulations: History and prospect. Landscape Journal, 6, 62–80.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gordon H. Orians .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this entry

Cite this entry

Orians, G.H. (2016). Savanna Hypothesis, The. In: Weekes-Shackelford, V., Shackelford, T., Weekes-Shackelford, V. (eds) Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_2930-1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_2930-1

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-16999-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Reference Behavioral Science and PsychologyReference Module Humanities and Social Sciences