Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science

Living Edition
| Editors: Todd K. Shackelford, Viviana A. Weekes-Shackelford

Butterfly Mimicry

  • Karin KjernsmoEmail author
Living reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_2672-1
  • 26 Downloads

Synonyms

Definition

Protective resemblance between a butterfly and two or more species, or between a butterfly and an inanimate object.

Introduction

Butterfly mimicry is a form of protective coloration where a given species, commonly referred to as the mimic, increases its chance of survival by visually resembling a harmful species, the model, such that the receiver of the signal, the predator (e.g., birds, reptiles, or predatory insects who attack and consume butterflies), gets confused between the two and avoids the mimic (Ruxton et al. 2004). The model can either be a different species of butterfly or an entirely different species of animal. In its broadest sense, butterfly mimicry may also involve a third category, a camouflage strategy known as Masquerade, in which butterflies mimic inanimate objects such as leaves, bits of lichen, or patches of tree bark (Ruxton et al. 2004). Traditionally, however, there are two main types of mimicry, and these are...

Keywords

Batesian Mimicry Unpalatable Species Unpalatable Prey Mimicry Ring Batesian Mimic 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.
This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References

  1. Bates, H. W. (1862). Contributions to an insect fauna of the Amazon valley Lepidoptera: Heliconiidae. Transactions of the Linnean Society of London, 23, 495–566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Benson, W. W. (1972). Natural selection for Müllerian mimicry in Heliconius erato in Costa Rica. Science 176, 936–939.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Finkbeiner, S. D., Briscoe, A. D., & Reed, R. D. (2012). The benefit of being a social butterfly: communal roosting deters predation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 279, 2769–2776.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  4. Kapan, D. D. (2001). Three-butterfly system provides a field test of Müllerian mimicry. Nature, 409, 338–340.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Mallet, J. (2001). Causes and consequences of a lack of coevolution in Müllerian mimicry. Evolutionary Ecology, 13, 777–806.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Mallet, J., & Gilbert, L. E. (1995). Why are there so many mimicry rings-correlations between habitat, behavior and mimicry in Heliconius butterflies. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 55, 159–180.Google Scholar
  7. Müller, F. 1879. Ituna and Thyridia; a remarkable case of mimicry in butterflies (trans: Meloda R). Transactions of the Entomological Society of London 20–29Google Scholar
  8. Ritland, D. B., & Bower, L. P. (1991). The viceroy is not a Batesian mimic. Nature, 350, 497–498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Ruxton, G. D., Sherrat, T. N., & Speed, M. P. (2004). Avoiding attack. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Speed, M. (1999). Batesian, quasi-Batesian or Müllerian mimicry? Theory and data in mimicry Research. Evolutionary Ecology, 13, 755–776.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Turner, J. R. G. (1984). Mimicry: the palatability spectrum and its consequences. In R. I. Vane-Wright & P. R. Ackery (Eds.), The Biology of Butterflies, Royal entomological society of London symposium (Vol. 11, pp. 141–161). New York: Academic.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of Biological SciencesUniversity of BristolBristolUK

Section editors and affiliations

  • Russell Jackson
    • 1
  1. 1.University of IdahoMoscowUSA