Encyclopedia of Autism Spectrum Disorders

2013 Edition
| Editors: Fred R. Volkmar

Global Versus Local Processing

  • Laurent Mottron
  • Isabelle Soulières
Reference work entry
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-1698-3_1732

Definition

Local and global processing refer to hierarchical dimensions within perceptual patterns. Any spatially or temporally extended structure can be dichotomized in its embedding component or global level, and its embedded components or local elements. Local and global dimensions are interdefined and are therefore only relative concepts: the same level can be considered as local relatively to its embedding structure and as global if it can itself be decomposed into embedded elements. The classic local–global tasks use “Navon-type” stimuli, which are large patterns (e.g., letters, geometrical figures) composed of small patterns of same or different nature (see Fig.  1). In the auditory modality, hierarchical stimuli can be implemented in melodies where local elements are single pitches or intervals, and global elements are melody or contour.
This is a preview of subscription content, log in to check access.

References and Readings

  1. Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P., & Faubert, J. (2005). Enhanced and diminished visuo-spatial information processing in autism depends on stimulus complexity. Brain, 128(10), 2430–2441.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bonnel, A., McAdams, S., Smith, B., Berthiaume, C., Bertone, A., Ciocca, V., et al. (2010). Enhanced pure-tone pitch discrimination among persons with autism but not Asperger syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 48(9), 2465–2475.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Caron, M. J., Mottron, L., Berthiaume, C., & Dawson, M. (2006). Cognitive mechanisms, specificity and neural underpinnings of visuospatial peaks in autism. Brain, 129(Pt 7), 1789–1802.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Creak, M. (1963). Childhood psychosis: A review of 100 cases. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 109, 84–89.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Frith, U. (1989). Autism : Explaining the enigma. Oxford, UK, Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
  6. Grill-Spector, K., & Malach, R. (2004). The human visual cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 649–677.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 5–25.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Hermelin, B., & O’Connor, N. (1970). Psychological experiments with autistic children. Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
  9. Kanner, L. (1951). The conception of wholes and parts in early infantile autism. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 108(1), 23–26.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Mottron, L., & Belleville, S. (1993). A study of perceptual analysis in a high-level autistic subject with exceptional graphic abilities. Brain and Cognition, 23(2), 279–309.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulieres, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update, and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 27–43.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Plaisted, K., O’Riordan, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Enhanced visual search for a conjunctive target in autism: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(5), 777–783.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Pourtois, G., Dan, E. S., Grandjean, D., Sander, D., & Vuilleumier, P. (2005). Enhanced extrastriate visual response to band pass spatial frequency filtered fearful faces: Time course and topographic evoked-potentials mapping. Human Brain Mapping, 26(1), 65–79.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Robertson, L. C., & Lamb, M. R. (1991). Neuropsychological contributions to theories of part/whole organization. Cognitive Psychology, 23(2), 299–330.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1983). An islet of ability in autistic children: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24(4), 613–620.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1993). Why do autistic individuals show superior performance on the block design task? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34(8), 1351–1364.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Soulières, I., Mottron, L., Saumier, D., & Larochelle, S. (2007). Atypical categorical perception in autism: Autonomy of discrimination? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(3), 481–490.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Vlamings, P. H., Jonkman, L. M., van Daalen, E., van der Gaag, R. J., & Kemner, C. (2010). Basic abnormalities in visual processing affect face processing at an early age in autism spectrum disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 68(12), 1107–1113.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Wang, L., Mottron, L., Peng, D., Berthiaume, C., & Dawson, M. (2007). Local bias and local-to-global interference without global deficit: A robust finding in autism under various conditions of attention, exposure time, and visual angle. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24(5), 550–574.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Center of Excellence in Pervasive Developmental Disorders of University of MontrealQCCanada
  2. 2.Department of PsychiatryRiviere-des-Prairies Hospital, University of MontrealMontrealCanada
  3. 3.Centre d’excellence en troubles envahissants du développement de l’université de MontréalHôpital Riviére-des-PrairiesMontréalCanada
  4. 4.Department of PsychologyUniversité du Québec à MontréalMontréalCanada